Participants
Our participants were drawn from the
Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (
ALSPAC) a British longitudinal cohort with a wealth of psychological, genetic, educational and health data spanning three decades [
17,
31,
32]. Pregnant women who were resident in Avon, UK with expected dates of delivery from 1st April 1991 to 31st December 1992 were invited to take part in the study. The initial number of pregnancies enrolled was 14,541, with an additional 913 eligible pregnancies added retrospectively in subsequent waves. These 15,454 pregnancies resulted in 15,589 foetuses, of which 14,901 were alive at 1 year of age. In the current study we screened for misophonia almost three decades later in Dec 2020- Feb 2021 as part of the “Life at 28+” wave of data collection, in over 4000 of the active remaining respondents from the index cohort (known as the ‘Children of the 90s Cohort”). Our participants with returned data were 4253 adults aged 28, comprising 1452 males-at-birth (mean age in months 345.81; SD 5.94), 2798 female-at-birth (mean age in months 345.49; SD 6.04) and 3 with no information about sex-at-birth (mean age in months 338.67; SD 4.51). The outcome of our screening for misophonia (see
Materials for screener) shows that this group contained 333 adults with misophonia (77 male and 256 female) and 3920 without misophonia (1375 male, 2542 female, 3 unknown). We refer to these as misophonics versus non-misophonics respectively (or misophonics vs. the comparison group). Of these individuals, Tables
1 and
2 show how many of these participants also had childhood data for our measures of interest. Table
1 shows the final number of participants in our DAWBA analyses (for ADHD, anxiety, depression), split by timepoints between 7–15 years. Table
2 shows the final participants for our sMFQ analyses for depression, again split by timepoint, but also by whichever person completed the questionnaire (child completed 10–16 years; parent completed 9–16 years).
Table 1
Participant numbers for our misophonic and non-misophonic participants, split by age at DAWBA completion. Also shown is a breakdown by sex-at-birth (female, male) with mean age (& standard deviation) in months for each timepoint
Total | 7 | 3296 | 2107 | 1189 | 91.79 (1.65) |
| 10 | 3304 | 2107 | 1197 | 128.54 (1.50) |
| 13 | 3215 | 2039 | 1176 | 166.83 (1.91) |
| 15 | 2569 | 1624 | 944 | 184.73 (2.88) |
Misophonics | 7 | 247 | 187 | 60 | 91.92 (2.01) |
| 10 | 238 | 181 | 57 | 128.48 (0.97) |
| 13 | 242 | 182 | 60 | 167.05 (2.44) |
| 15 | 194 | 150 | 44 | 184.77 (2.75) |
Non-misophonics | 7 | 3049 | 1920 | 1129 | 91.78 (1.61) |
| 10 | 3066 | 1926 | 1140 | 128.54 (1.53) |
| 13 | 2973 | 1857 | 1116 | 166.82 (1.87) |
| 15 | 2374 | 1474 | 900 | 184.73 (2.90) |
Table 2
Participant numbers split by age at sMFQ completion (both child-completed and parent-completed versions). Also shown is a breakdown by sex-at-birth (female, male) with mean age (& standard deviation) in months for each timepoint
Total Parent-completed | 9 | 3315 | 2128 | 1187 | 116 (1.42) |
| 11 | 3169 | 2034 | 1134 | 141 (1.50) |
| 13 | 3168 | 2019 | 1149 | 158 (2.00) |
| 16 | 2791 | 1759 | 1032 | 202 (4.32) |
Misophonics | 9 | 248 | 186 | 62 | 116 (1.62) |
| 11 | 241 | 186 | 55 | 141 (1.90) |
| 13 | 231 | 172 | 59 | 158 (1.99) |
| 16 | 205 | 158 | 47 | 202 (4.24) |
Non-misophonics | 9 | 3067 | 1942 | 1125 | 116 (1.40) |
| 11 | 2928 | 1848 | 1080 | 141 (1.46) |
| 13 | 2937 | 1847 | 1090 | 158 (2.00) |
| 16 | 2586 | 1601 | 985 | 202 (4.32) |
Total Child-completed | 10 | 3241 | 2086 | 1155 | 127 (2.87) |
| 12 | 3154 | 2026 | 1128 | 153 (2.62) |
| 13 | 2966 | 1878 | 1088 | 166 (2.35) |
| 16 | 2898 | 1944 | 954 | 200 (2.79) |
Misophonics | 10 | 254 | 192 | 62 | 128 (2.74) |
| 12 | 239 | 184 | 55 | 154 (3.11) |
| 13 | 227 | 174 | 53 | 166 (2.69) |
| 16 | 224 | 182 | 42 | 200 (2.74) |
Non-misophonics | 10 | 2987 | 1894 | 1093 | 127 (2.88) |
| 12 | 2915 | 1842 | 1073 | 153 (2.58) |
| 13 | 2739 | 1704 | 1035 | 166 (2.32) |
| 16 | 2674 | 1762 | 912 | 200 (2.79) |
Materials
The Development and Wellbeing Assessment (DAWBA; [
29]). This parent-completed questionnaire poses a series of clinically-based questions about the child, relating to symptoms indicative of mental health conditions. Parents completed this questionnaire at multiple timepoints, specifically when their child was 7, 10, 13 and 15 years. The DAWBA score indicates the likelihood of being diagnosed with a series of mental health conditions based on the criteria of the
International Classification of Diseases-10 (ICD-10) and the
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders fourth edition (DSM-IV). Our hypotheses led us to consider three mental health conditions, which were available across all four time points: ADHD, depression, and anxiety disorder. For ADHD, there were 22 items, which began with a yes/no question (
Over the last 6 months… do you think your child definitely has some problems with overactivity of poor concentration?) followed by 21 items on a response scale running from 0 to 2 (e.g.,
Have their teachers complained over the last 6 months of problems with poor concentration or being easily distracted? No, or doesn’t apply; A little; A lot). For depression, there were 17 items, comprising 12 yes/no questions (e.g.,
In the last 4 weeks, have there been times when your child has been very sad, miserable, unhappy or tearful?), as well as two items on a 3-point scale (e.g.,
When your child has been miserable, could they be cheered up? Easily; With difficulty/ only briefly; Not at all) and three items on a 2-point scale (e.g.,
Over the last 4 weeks, the period of being really miserable has lasted less than 2 weeks; 2 weeks or more?). Finally, the DAWBA also indicated the likelihood of clinical diagnosis for anxiety disorder. This comprised a package of interviews identifying one or more anxiety disorder from among generalised anxiety disorder, social phobia, separation anxiety, PTSD, OCD, and specific phobias (e.g.,
Over the last 6 months has your child worried excessively on more days than not?). The full list of items and scoring is described in detail for all conditions at
https://www.dawba.info/py/dawbainfo/b4.py (see also[
29,
33,
34] ).
The DAWBA is a widely-accepted measure described in over 400 publications, and is well validated in both clinical [
35] and epidemiological studies [
34]. For example, it shows excellent discrimination between clinic and community samples in rates of diagnosed disorders [
29] and substantial agreement between DAWBA and case note diagnoses. It has been translated into at least 20 languages (
https://www.dawba.info/py/dawbainfo/b4.py), and has been used by national statistics agencies to survey nationwide child psychiatric morbidities (e.g., the UK National Statistics; [
33,
36]). In our own sample we examined test-restest reliability, finding a ‘good’ intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) of 0.85 for ADHD, a ‘moderate’ ICC for anxiety (0.59) although poorer for depression (0.38; [
37]). However, our test-retest period was exceptionally long, spanning almost a decade from 7 to 16 years, whereas the ICC is most often applied to test-retest conducted over substantially shorter intervals [
37].
The Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (sMFQ). This 13-item measure probes depression symptoms over the two weeks prior to testing (e.g.,
In the past two weeks… felt miserable or unhappy?). Responses are given a 3-point Likert scale from
Not at all (scored 0),
Sometimes true (scored 1) or
True (scored 2), and the test is scored out of 26 with higher scores representing more depressive traits. There were both child- and parent-completed versions of the test. Parents completed this questionnaire when their children were 9,11,13 and 16 years, and the equivalent childhood span for the child/self-completed version was 10, 12, 13 and 16 years. The sMFQ is a validated measure of cognitive and affective depressive symptomology, and can successfully discriminate clinically depressed from general population samples in children 8–16 years [
37], and depressed from non-depressed children within the general population (where depression was independently identified with the Diagnostic Interview Schedule depression scale; [
38])
2. We measured test-retest reliability in our sample and found an ICC of 0.66 for child-completed sMFQ, and an ICC of 0.75 for parent-completed sMFQ, being ‘moderate’ and ‘moderate-to-good’ respectively [
39].
Sussex Screener for Misophonia (SSfM). We developed and administered this written screener as a measure for adult misophonia. The SSfM provided examples of known misophonia triggers (eating noises; throat clearing; nasal noises; etc.) as well as a characterisation of misophonia to which participants agreed or disagreed (i.e., When sounds (e.g., crunching) consistently cause extreme emotions, like anger, disgust or anxiety). For those agreeing with misophonia, our screener contained further items probing its severity in disrupting daily life (not at all, very mildly, moderately, severely, very severely) and additional questions for consideration elsewhere, such as whether the participant had sought clinical support for their misophonia, and when they believe it developed (early school [up to 11]; later school [11 to 18], adulthood [18 + years]). Misophonics were classified as those agreeing with our characterisation of misophonia, but also rejecting ‘not at all’ for its detriment to daily life (i.e., misophonia must disrupt daily life for participants in our misophonia group, at least to some degree). The remainder (i.e., those without any detrimental misophonia) formed our comparison group.
To have confidence that our screener corrected identifies people with misophonia, we first confirmed it converges with multiple lines of evidence for misophonia. Here, we compared it to evidence gathered from assessments administered to the same cohort, including a clinic audiology visit carried out when the cohort were 11 years old [
40]. Although misophonia had not been classified or even named at the time of this earlier clinic, there are clear indicators we can use. For example, adults identified with misophonia using our screener were twice as likely than the comparison group to report a sound sensitivity in the clinic assessment at age 11 years (i.e., “Do you ever experience over-sensitivity or distress to particular sounds?”; 6.5% misophonics vs. 3.5% non-misophonics,
χ2 (1) = 4.93,
p = .026). They were also more than twice as likely to be wearing ear defenders at age 11 to protect themselves from aversive sounds compared to their peers (12.5% vs. 5.05%). Although numbers here were too small to test this statistically (since only 16 misophonics were given this latter question
3), we can also see evidence of misophonia in other converging traits. Hence, our misophonia group were significantly more likely to dislike eating in the presence of others at age 13 (remembering that the very strongest triggers of misophonia are other people’s eating sounds). Here, 5.10% of misophonics disliked eating with others a little or a lot compared to only 2.80% of the comparison group (Fishers exact
p = .024). At age 25, misophonics were still significantly less likely to eat with others, and instead spent significantly more time than the comparison group eating alone. For example, 13.82% of misophonics ate alone 5 + times during the last week, compared to only 10.81% of the comparison group. This difference is significant (i.e., using the response-scale running from 0 [i.e., never eating alone] to 4 [i.e., 7 + times eating alone], misophonics had a mean of M = 1.42, SD = 1.24; compared to non-misophonics M = 1.16, SD = 1.16;
t(254.75) = -3.29,
p = .002, Cohen’s
d = 0.23). At the same age, misophonics were also significantly more likely to use sound-distractions at the dinner table (e.g., television playing whilst eating) compared with non-misophonics (i.e., mean days without distraction was 1.00 days per week for misophonics [SD 1.18] vs. 1.18 for non-misophonics, SD = 1.20;
t(261.97) = 2.13,
p = .034, Cohen’s
d = 0.15). Finally, at age 25, people classified by our screener as misophonic were also overwhelmingly more likely to indicate typical inter-personal difficulties expected from misophonia (e.g., “In last 6 months compared to people of the same age, I am easily annoyed by others”); i.e., 71.93% misophonics indicated somewhat or certainly true compared to 49.52% of non-misophonics (with respective means on the full scale scored 0–2 [
not true,
somewhat true,
certainly true] as follows: M = 0.94, SD = 0.71; M = 0.57, SD = 0.63;
t(174.04) = -6.01,
p < .001, Cohens
d = 0.58). Finally, we also selected a feature to examine for
divergent validity, i.e., a feature expected to show
no difference for misophonics and non-misophonics. As expected from the literature [
16], we found that our misophonia screener was entirely non-predictive of
creative self-concept, a well-studied self-assessment of one’s own creative ability [
41,
42] which correlates with direct measurements of creativity, but is known to show no difference between misophonics and non-misophonics [
16]. Hence, using an almost identical question to previous misophonia literature (“How good are you at art?”; rated from
not good at all to
very good) we found that the creative self-concept of misophonics (M = 3.67, SD = 1.18) was no different to the comparison group (M = 3.57, SD = 1.08;
t(289.23) = 1.23,
p = .220, Cohen’s
d = 0.09), thereby providing divergent validity for our measure of misophonia.
In summary, our screener has both convergent and divergent validity, since participants identified as having misophonia were significantly more likely to have reported a sound sensitivity at age 11, were significantly more likely to avoid eating near others by parental report at 13, and again by self-report at age 25, and were significantly more likely to use sound-distractions while eating (e.g., tv playing). They were also more likely to have been annoyed by others in the 6 months previously, and they explicitly endorsed a careful description of misophonia at the age of 28, while agreeing that it impacted their lives. Finally, as expected, they were no different to the comparison group in their creative self-concept. This ample convergent and divergent validity for our measure leads us to conclude that our screener successfully detects adults with misophonia.
Procedure
Our SSfM (misophonia screener) was administered as part of ALSPAC’s “Life at 28+” wave of data collection. Participants completed the screener online in digital form, with a pencil-and-paper version made available where requested. The screener took approximately 5-minutes to complete. These data were collected and managed by the ALSPAC team using Redcap, a secure web-based software platform hosted at the University of Bristol, designed to support data capture for research studies [
43]. We subsequently accessed our data, alongside existing data from the DAWBA and the sMFQ, which were shared from ALSPAC’s back-catalogue of data from the 1990-2000 s. ALSPAC’s fully searchable data dictionary and variable search tool can be found online at
http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/our-data.
Analytic Plan. We examined longitudinal data from the DAWBA (i.e., probability of meeting the diagnostic criteria for ADHD, anxiety disorder and depression) and the sMFQ (symptoms of depression). The DAWBA has an ordinal scale with unequal distances between scale points (e.g., 1 = < 0.1% probability of diagnosis, 2 = ~ 0.5% probability of diagnosis, 3 = ~ 3% probability of diagnosis etc.). We therefore applied a non-parametric test using the
nparLD package in R [
44] which produces ANOVA-type statistics (and also Wald-type statistics, which held the same interpretation for all our results). We present our ANOVA statistics below, henceforth referred to as non-parametric ANOVA. Our models examine differences between groups (misophonics vs. non-misophonics) across four timepoints (7, 10, 13 and 15 years), for three different diagnoses (anxiety disorder, ADHD, depression). Hence we perform three separate analyses, with the outcome being diagnosis likelihood of anxiety disorder, ADHD, and depression, respectively. While ADD/ADHD symptoms decrease with age and are higher in males [
45], both anxiety and depression increase with age around the middle of adolescence, and start to emerge more strongly in females from around age 13 [
46,
47]. These periods correspond to the upper tail of our age range, so we therefore additionally added sex-at-birth as a predictor (which was provided in our data as male/female). We remind the reader that effect sizes cannot be directly measured for non-parametric mixed ANOVAs [
48]. Although some mental health domains also have sub-conditions (e.g., anxiety disorder includes sub-conditions of social anxiety, separation anxiety, specific phobia etc.) we look at domain-level diagnosis. For example, we examine the likelihood of developing anxiety disorder, whether that be social anxiety, separation anxiety, or specific phobias etc. We took this approach following earlier literature e.g., [
49] and because fine-grained analyses would be under-powered due to the relative rarity of specific sub-conditions during the period we examine (7–16 years). Finally, we use Mann Whitney Wilcoxons to explore pairwise post-hoc comparisons on ordinal data, correcting for multiple comparisons.
Scores from the sMFQ (depression traits) are continuous but due to non-normality we also ran non-parametric ANOVA using the same framework as before. We analysed child-completed and parent-completed sMFQ scores in separate analyses since agreement between child and parent reports can sometimes be rather low [
50,
51]. For the child-completed model, we examined differences between groups (misophonics vs. non-misophonics) across four timepoints (10, 12, 13 and 16 years) and there were also four timepoints for our parent-competed model (9, 11, 13 and 16 years)
4. All analyses were performed in R 3.6.3 using R Studio, with the R packages
tidyverse for general data wrangling,
ggplot2 for figures,
nparLD for non-parametric longitudinal analyses.