Test anxiety is considered a situation-specific personality trait triggered by evaluative stimuli (Zeidner & Matthews,
2003). In research literature, it is conceptualized as a multi-dimensional construct, most commonly based on facets of worry and emotionality. Worry refers to the cognitive component, including task-irrelevant thinking and deprecatory thoughts, while emotionality refers to increased physiological arousal, such as nervousness and tension. Both components are thought to reduce task-focused attention and impair performance, highlighting their importance for students’ academic success (Liebert & Morris,
1967). While research has demonstrated the impact of peers and parents for upper secondary or university students, less is known about younger children, particularly during the transition to secondary school. Additionally, there is little research on whether these social factors impact girls’ and boys’ test anxiety differently. Identifying predictors of test anxiety is critical, as it can limit children’s long-term academic development and well-being (Robson et al.,
2023; von der Embse et al.,
2018). Understanding these predictors during the transition period may be relevant for interventions targeting test anxiety. The aim of the present study is therefore to examine the impact of peers and parents on girls’ and boys’ test anxiety during the transition to secondary school.
Secondary School Transition and Test Anxiety
“Surviving the Junior High School Transition”—the title of a transition study (Lord et al.,
1994)—emphasizes the significance of the secondary school transition in a child’s life. The transition is marked by increased performance demands and major changes in classroom characteristics (Sirsch,
2003). In several European countries, such as Germany, Austria, Croatia, and Portugal, this transition occurs after 4th grade (Coelho et al.,
2020; for an overview of European educational systems, see also Hoerner et al.,
2015). It coincides with the period when children begin to incorporate evaluative feedback into their self-perceptions. Experiences of failure during this period can lead to a long-term fear of failure (Wigfield & Eccles,
1989). Research indicates that students who transition earlier (e.g., after the 5th grade compared to after the 6th grade), tend to develop more school-related anxiety in the long term (Grills-Taquechel et al.,
2010).
While previous research about school transitions mostly examined the post-transition period, little is known about the importance of the pre-transition period for the development of test anxiety. Educational assessments in late elementary school can be considered
critical moments, as they are highly relevant for students’ future educational careers (Flitcroft et al.,
2017). For instance, in the German school system, a recommendation for the type of secondary school is made based on grades obtained in the last period of elementary school. Because studies indicate that perceived test importance fosters test anxiety (Eklöf & Nyroos,
2013; von der Embse et al.,
2018), the time before the school transition, characterized by an increased emphasis on school grades, may be relevant for the development of test anxiety. In line with this, studies suggest that various types of anxiety are highest before the transition and decrease afterward (Symonds & Galton,
2014). Since school-related evaluative anxiety develops as a function of the respective school type and school performance (Valtin & Wagner,
2004), another possible explanation is that low-performing children in particular benefit from the transition, as their new social group tends to have similar levels of achievement (big-fish-little-pond effect, Marsh & Parker,
1984). To date, there is a lack of studies exploring the mean values and stability of test anxiety during the transition from elementary to secondary school in the context of early transition (after the 4th grade).
After the transition, children are confronted with new academic demands and school subjects, an increased emphasis on grading and social competition, as well as a new social learning environment with new classmates and a changing school size (Lohaus et al.,
2004; Valtin & Wagner,
2004). Adapting to these academic changes and renegotiating their status in their new peer group requires a great deal of psychological and behavioral adjustment (Kingery et al.,
2011; Langenkamp,
2010). On the one hand, these challenges provide children with the opportunity to learn and develop the skills they need for a successful academic career (Skinner & Raine,
2023). Thus, most students seem to adjust to their new environment without serious difficulties or even experience the transition as something positive (Lohaus et al.,
2004; Sirsch,
2003). On the other hand, when the demands exceed their resources, children may struggle with these changes and experience declines in their developmental trajectories, such as their motivational orientation or self-confidence (Grolnick et al.,
2000). Therefore, it might be worthwhile to take a differentiated perspective on children’s initial conditions and resources in late elementary school years that facilitate psychological adjustment to the new school context (Sirsch,
2003). In this context, the motivational theory of coping assumes that children’s interpersonal resources, such as parental or peer support are a crucial factor (Skinner & Wellborn,
1997). As previous research mostly examined the effects of peer (Hoferichter & Raufelder,
2015; Song et al.,
2015) or parental support (Bouffard & Labranche,
2022; Song et al.,
2015) on test anxiety from the 7th grade, empirical evidence on the predictors of test anxiety at the time of early transition (after the 4th grade) is generally lacking. A detailed understanding of the predictors in this particular age group might be beneficial to identify critical phases and antecedents of test anxiety development.
Role of Peers
Considering that children spend a large part of their daytime at school, it does not seem surprising that peer relationships in their new class context are highly relevant for children’s psychological well-being and school adjustment. For instance, studies indicate the importance of the peer group for students’ academic achievement (Sebanc et al.,
2016), school involvement (Kingery et al.,
2011), and test and manifest anxiety in secondary school (Grills & Ollendick,
2002; Hoferichter & Raufelder,
2015; Rubin et al.,
2015). Since it fulfills the basic need for relatedness, feeling accepted by the new peer group is considered a particular resource in dealing with life challenges such as the school transition (Langenkamp,
2010; Zimmer-Gembeck et al.,
2023). Accordingly, even mild stressors can trigger anxiety and lead to maladaptive outcomes in children who feel threatened in their basic need for relatedness (Skinner & Wellborn,
1997).
At the age of school transition, relationships with peers become increasingly important (Bokhorst et al.,
2010; Zimmer-Gembeck et al.,
2023). However, the transition is accompanied by drastic changes in children’s social environment: Since former classmates often attend different schools, the children have to deal with new classmates and a new social reference group with a greater emphasis on social comparison (Bouffard & Labranche,
2022; Kingery et al.,
2011). Hence, friendships in the new class are one of the key factors contributing to children’s sense of well-being (Curson et al.,
2019). In contrast, a possible mismatch between the need for peer acceptance and actual peer integration after the transition may be particularly related to adjustment difficulties (Kingery et al.,
2011). To date, the link between peer relationships and test anxiety during the transition to secondary school has received little attention so far. One study found a negative link between peer support and test anxiety after the transition (7th grade) to secondary school in a Korean sample (Song et al.,
2015). However, this finding can only be applied to a limited extent to Western cultures due to cultural differences (Robson et al.,
2023; Song et al.,
2015). In addition, the time of the transition differs: While children change school after 4th grade in some European states, the transition in Korea happened after the 6th grade (Song et al.,
2015). This is particularly worth mentioning, given the different development phases children may experience at this time. Thus, some research findings suggest that students who make the transition earlier may experience more pronounced difficulties than their older peers because their emotional resources may not yet be sufficient to deal with these changes (Grills-Taquechel et al.,
2010). Therefore, it might be important to examine the factors that contribute to a successful transition in this particular age group.
Role of Parents
During stressful life events, the family plays an important role as the primary source of comfort and support. For example, when a child is dealing with school demands like bad grades, parental reactions can determine what resources are available to regulate the event. Parental disciplinary measures, such as scolding or blaming, can further exacerbate the difficult situation and make maladaptive academic outcomes more likely (Skinner & Wellborn,
1997; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner,
2016). Despite the well-established assumption that parents have a significant impact on their children’s academic beliefs and behaviors, relatively little attention has been paid to the specific role they can play in facilitating their children’s successful academic adjustment during the transition to secondary school (Bouffard & Labranche,
2022).
In the tradition of socialization research, parental discipline practices are often classified as
parental control and
parental autonomy support (Ryan & Deci,
2000). Parental control is characterized by pressure and threats of punishment and thus appears to reduce children’s confidence in their ability to handle difficult situations on their own (Wigfield et al.,
2015). There is ample empirical evidence that parental control is associated with anxiety (Ballash et al.,
2006; Duchesne & Ratelle,
2010; Luis et al.,
2008). Thus, test anxiety is thought to originate from negative parental reactions to performance in evaluative settings (Putwain et al.,
2010). In secondary school (Ringeisen & Raufelder,
2015; Ritchwood et al.,
2015) or college students (Putwain et al.,
2010, Shadach & Ganor-Miller,
2013), parental control was found to be related to test anxiety, especially to cognitive test anxiety (Putwain et al.,
2010; Shadach & Ganor-Miller,
2013). Research on younger children, especially at the time of the secondary school transition, is lacking. As previous findings are mostly based on cross-sectional studies (e.g., Putwain et al.,
2010; Ringeisen & Raufelder,
2015), longitudinal effects remain unexplored.
In contrast to parental control, research on parental autonomy support differs in its conceptualizations. Some studies measure autonomy support as the dimensionally opposite pole of parental control (e.g., Grolnick et al.,
2000). Recent conceptualizations of parental autonomy support encompass more than the absence of control, including aspects such as acknowledging the perspectives of the children, providing explanations, and offering choices whenever possible (McCurdy, et al.,
2020; Zimmer-Gembeck & Skinner,
2016). In this way, children learn how to handle difficult situations and take responsibility for themselves (Spinrad et al.,
2023). Studies based on this conceptualization suggest positive associations with academic adjustment and psychological well-being (for an overview, see Vasquez et al.,
2016). Transferred to the time of transition, it is assumed that children who experience themselves as more autonomous also develop adaptive strategies to deal with the new challenges (Grolnick et al.,
2000). To date, there are no studies on the relation between parental autonomy support and test anxiety during transition to secondary school. Examining this relation may deepen the understanding of specific parental behaviors that affect test anxiety.
Gender Differences
While there is compelling evidence that girls report more test anxiety (Putwain & Daly,
2014; Ritchwood et al.,
2015; Robson et al.,
2023), more social support from parents and friends (Bokhorst et al.,
2010; Colarossi & Eccles,
2003), and less parental control (Duchesne & Ratelle,
2010), there is little research on whether social factors have a sex-differential effect on test anxiety. In general, research findings indicate that girls are more concerned about their dyadic relationships and peer evaluation, report more stress in their peer group, and rely more on social support to cope with stressors (Rose & Rudolph,
2006). Moreover, girls tend to establish intimate friendships at an earlier stage of adolescence than boys and are more likely to experience higher levels of intimacy in these relationships. This intimacy may render girls more vulnerable when their relationships are disrupted. Indeed, girls exhibit greater levels of anxiety than boys in response to stressors in their interpersonal relationships (Kingery et al.,
2011; Rose & Rudolph,
2006). As a possible consequence, the negative effects of transitions are thought to be greater for girls than for boys (Anderson et al.,
2000). While these results provide evidence that girls’ well-being is generally more affected by social factors, particularly their peer relations, empirical literature examining gender-differential effects in test anxiety is limited. Two studies found no gender differences in the relation between peer and parental support and test anxiety (Hoferichter & Raufelder,
2015; Song et al.,
2015). Since both studies examined gender differences in adolescents from 7th grade, little is known about gender differences in younger children, especially at the time of early school transition (after the 4th grade) as it occurs in most German federal states. Understanding whether differential factors contribute to boys’ and girls’ test anxiety could be highly relevant for intervention programs.