Introduction
Adolescence is a developmental period characterized by rapid biological, social, and emotional changes including increased autonomy, more time spent with peers, and changes in relationships with parents and other caregivers (Steinberg,
2014). Substance use is typically initiated during this time and is associated with an increased likelihood of developing substance use disorders later in life (Gray & Squeglia,
2018). In the United States, 15% of adolescents meet criteria for alcohol abuse, and 16% meet the criteria for drug abuse by age 18 (Swendsen et al.,
2012). The effects of substance use during adolescence can be long lasting in part due to neurotoxin exposure which can have detrimental effects on cognition and emotional functioning and increase potential for substance use disorders and addiction in adulthood (Brown et al.,
2008). From a neurobiological perspective, during adolescence, brain regions responsible for lower order emotion processing functions mature sooner than frontal regions associated with higher order cognitive functioning (Casey et al.,
2008). The imbalance between these emotional reactivity and cognitive control systems renders adolescents particularly vulnerable to risk-taking behaviors such as substance use (Casey et al.,
2008; Steinberg,
2008). Relatedly, the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment model (Kwako et al.,
2018) offers a more specific dimensional approach to understanding substance use by outlining three domains implicated in addiction that overlap with three of the six Research Domain Criteria (RDoC; Insel, 2010). These domains are (1) incentive salience (feelings of reward from stimuli associated with substances), (2) negative affect (emotional state characterized by unpleasant or lack of feelings), and (3) executive function (higher-order cognitive abilities such as inhibition, set shifting, and working memory; Kwako et al.,
2018). The present study applied the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment model to understand the transactional nature of risk and protective factors for substance use development during adolescence, viewing the heterogeneity of substance use as a function of the interface between emotional risk (incentive salience and negative affect) and cognitive protective (executive function) factors. In doing so, sensation seeking in adolescents was assessed as a proxy for incentive salience, given the relevance of sensation seeking in reflecting adolescents’ approach to substance use (i.e., sensation seeking is correlated with reward sensitivity; Harden et al.,
2018). Prior research has noted moderate to strong associations between reward sensitivity and sensation seeking in adolescents (Scott-Parker et al.,
2012). Additionally, neuroimaging work provides evidence for associations between heightened neural response to rewards and sensation seeking in late adolescence (Hawes et al.,
2017), thus providing rationale for using sensation seeking in the present study.
In the current literature, the three domains of the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment are implicated in substance use progression in various ways. High sensation seeking contributes to vulnerability for substance use (LaSpada et al.,
2020). For individuals with high negative affect, substance use may be a coping mechanism to relieve feelings of negativity (Magee & Connell,
2021). The evidence for direct effects of executive function on substance use is mixed, and may vary with effect size (Aytaclar et al.,
1999). More convincing evidence suggests that executive function interacts with other emotion or motivation variables such as negative affect and sensation seeking to predict substance use (Kim-Spoon et al.,
2017). For example, associations between substance use, frustration, and reward sensitivity are weaker for adolescents with high cognitive control, compared to those with low cognitive control (Kim-Spoon et al.,
2016), highlighting the regulatory role of executive function.
To better understand the etiology of substance use, prior work has proposed internalizing and externalizing pathways to substance use problems in adolescence, suggesting two distinct categories of adolescents who may be especially vulnerable to substance use. The externalizing pathway underscores the role of delinquent behavior, which is often driven by sensation seeking (Edwards et al.,
2016), whereas the internalizing pathway emphasizes negative affect and self-medication as potential factors contributing to substance use (Hussong et al.,
2011). Importantly, these theorized pathways may operate at both within- and between-person levels, such that having higher negative affect or sensation seeking than usual for an individual may lead to increased substance use (within-person process). Additionally, those with higher negative affect or sensation seeking on average may use substances more than those with lower negative affect (between-person process). Although these processes have been tested primarily at the between-person level, a few studies have also examined the associations between negative affect or sensation seeking and substance use at both the between- and within-person level. In a sample of Chinese late adolescents, there were positive bi-directional year-to-year associations between sensation seeking and substance use (combined cigarette and alcohol use) at the within-person level as well as a positive association between sensation seeking change and substance use change over three years at the between-person level (Shen et al.,
2023). Similarly, another study found both between- and within- person associations between sensation seeking and alcohol use in a national sample of adolescents from the United States (Waddell & Sasser,
2022). Conversely, in another sample, only within-person associations between sensation seeking and binge drinking were observed (Waddell & Chassin,
2023). Given these mixed findings, it is important to further investigate the associations between sensation seeking and substance use at both within-person and between-person levels. In a study of U.S. late adolescents, albeit not directly assessing negative affect, there were positive bi-directional year-to-year associations between depression and alcohol coping motives at the within-person level, as well as a positive concurrent association between depression and alcohol coping motives at the between-person level (Colder et al.,
2019). In this study, depression was not associated with alcohol consumption at either the between-person or within-person level.
Understanding the roles of negative affect and sensation seeking on substance use at the within-person level is theoretically crucial for two reasons: First, the motivational processes (i.e., negative affect and sensation seeking) of the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment model are within-person determinants of the pathways to substance use progression and addiction (e.g., elevated negative affect/sensation seeking predicts future engagement in substance use). As such, modeling within-person processes is arguably the most relevant for better understanding developmental processes whereby negative affect and sensation seeking contribute to substance use progression. Second, between- and within-person effects may differ, and thus disaggregating their distinct effects is important. For example, prior studies focusing only on between-person associations demonstrated that adolescents with higher levels of negative affect are more likely to engage in substance use to cope with negative affect, and substance use is associated with higher negative affect on average (Mason et al.,
2009; Measelle et al.,
2006). In contrast, a recent meta-analysis summarizing within-person negative affect- substance use associations suggested non-significant effects of negative affect on substance use (measured via alcohol use) when daily diary approaches were utilized (Dora et al.,
2023). Yet, traditional statistical methods blend between- and within-person effects, making results difficult to interpret (Berry & Willoughby,
2017). Better understanding of how negative affect and sensation seeking relate to adolescent substance use at both within- and between-person levels will aid in preventive interventions for problematic substance use among youth and adolescents by clarifying the target population (i.e., between-person prediction) and identifying risk factors for substance use and progression toward addiction (i.e., within-person prediction).
Discussion
Prior work has identified negative affect, sensation seeking, and executive function as biobehavioral markers of addiction (Kwako et al.,
2018), and these factors have also been associated with adolescent substance use, which may precede later addiction (Giancola & Mezzich,
2003; Hussong et al.,
2011). Nonetheless, the extent to which these factors influence substance use development at the within-person level, between-person level, or both has not previously been systematically examined. In addition, prior research has examined the contributions of negative affect, sensation seeking, and executive function primarily at the between-person level. The present study filled these significant gaps in the literature by examining how negative affect, sensation seeking, and executive function—the three biobehavioral domains of the Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment model—contribute to the individual differences in developmental trajectories of substance use progression throughout adolescence. Understanding bidirectional associations between negative affect/sensation seeking and substance use and moderating roles of executive function that predate clinical levels of substance use is crucial. This insight reveals how problematic organizations of the cognitive and emotional systems can lead to later substance use disorders and addiction, providing essential factors to target in prevention programs (Cicchetti,
2010). Latent curve models with structured residuals examining between- and-within-person bidirectional associations among negative affect and sensation seeking with substance use, at varying levels of executive function were estimated. Results indicated that higher negative affect was associated with higher subsequent substance use and higher substance use was associated with higher subsequent negative affect, for adolescents with lower executive function. Higher sensation seeking at baseline (age 14) was associated with increasing substance use from age 14 to 17, regardless of executive function level.
The present study found significant increases in the mean levels of substance use across adolescence as well as individual differences in the developmental trajectories, replicating prior findings that have identified adolescence as a time of increased substance use and significant individual variability in substance use rates (Johnston et al.,
2016). In contrast, the present study found non-significant developmental changes on average in negative affect and sensation seeking. For negative affect, this finding is consistent with prior longitudinal work using community samples of adolescents reporting non-significant developmental trends across adolescence, (Griffith et al.,
2021; Weinstein et al.,
2007). Based on these findings and prior findings of decreasing negative affect in childhood (e.g., Murphy et al.,
1999), developmental changes in negative affect may be stabilized by early adolescence (i.e., before age 14), showing non-significant change during mid-late adolescence (i.e., during ages 14–17). For sensation seeking, the present study found no significant change across time. This finding corresponds with prior work that has identified curvilinear trends of sensation seeking throughout adolescence, with levels increasing during early adolescence before stabilizing during mid-adolescence (i.e., 14–17 years) and subsequently declining throughout young adulthood (Steinberg et al.,
2018; Harden & Tucker-Drob,
2011).
In the negative affect-substance use model, significant effects emerged at the within-person level only. Specifically, for the low executive function group only, a bidirectional link between substance use and negative affect was found. Increased negative affect was associated with increased substance use the following year, and increased substance use was associated with subsequent increased negative affect. This finding extends prior longitudinal work showing that elevated negative affect is associated with increased use of alcohol to cope with negative feelings, and that the association between negative affect and substance use is reciprocal such that higher negative affect predicts more substance use and more substance use predicts higher negative affect at the within-person level (Mason et al.,
2009; Measelle et al.,
2006). This association was present only for individuals with lower executive function, and primarily at the within-person level, underscoring the role of executive function in the negative affect to substance use link. Additionally, the significant bidirectional association aligns with past findings and theoretical models suggesting that high levels of negative affect lead to higher risk of drug use as a coping mechanism (Hogarth,
2020) and that individuals continue using substances to avoid higher negative affect (Kassel et al.,
2007). Alternatively, these findings suggest that continued substance use increases subsequent negative affect in adolescents. Additionally, these findings support the neurobiological disease model of substance use, which emphasizes that with the progression of substance use, the role of pleasure-seeking changes (Koob & Moal,
1997; Volkow,
2005). The present findings expand upon this theoretical perspective and provide evidence for the reciprocal nature of the negative affect-substance use link. Prior studies on between-person associations between negative affect and substance use show evidence for this negative affect to substance use path (Bradley et al.,
2011; Measelle et al.,
2006), whereas studies of
daily within-person associations document no significant effect of negative affect on substance use (e.g., alcohol; Dora et al.,
2023). The present study results suggest that the negative affect-substance use year-to-year association is bidirectional at the within-person level, suggesting a more distal (i.e., yearly) cyclical pattern of negative affect and substance use, particularly among low executive function adolescents.
These findings highlight the important role of negative affect in substance use progression and provide support for the internalizing pathway to substance use. During adolescence, evidence for an internalizing pathway to substance use seems to be less prominent than evidence for an externalizing pathway to substance use in the literature, however the prior studies examined these associations solely at the between-person level (Hussong et al.,
2017; Rothenberg et al.,
2020). It may not be that those who are higher in negative affect
on average use substances more often. Rather, these data suggest that this pathway is stronger at the within-person level, such that an individual’s elevated negative affect relates to increased substance use, particularly for those with low executive function. Importantly, neuroimaging work has identified key brain regions implicated in impaired regulation of negative affect. Specifically, the amygdala, insula, and prefrontal cortex (PFC) are involved in the regulation and processing of emotions (Wager et al.,
2008), and weak resting state connectivity between the amygdala, insula, and regulatory regions of the brain have been shown in individuals with substance use disorders (Wilcox et al.,
2016). Collectively, neuroimaging work and the present findings converge to indicate the internalizing pathway to substance use at both the neural and behavioral levels.
An important contribution of the present study is the examination of the role of executive function in adolescent substance use. Research has shown that executive function and related constructs (i.e., cognitive control) moderate the effects of negative affect. For example, compared to individuals with higher executive function, individuals with lower executive function are more likely to ruminate and focus on negative life events, which is associated with elevated depressive symptoms (Gotlib & Joormann,
2010). In the present study, the effect of negative affect was present only among the adolescents with low executive function who may lack the necessary regulatory skills to cope with higher than usual negative affect, and therefore may be more likely to escalate substance use to assuage negative feelings. This finding expands upon the existing literature, demonstrating that executive function may be a protective factor in the internalizing pathway to substance use (i.e., in the presence of negative affect). As such, our findings underscore that negative affect heightens adolescents’ vulnerability to substance use progression, and executive function—a modifiable individual difference factor (Diamond & Ling,
2020)—attenuates this vulnerability.
Turning to the models for sensation seeking and substance use, at the between-person level, higher sensation seeking on average at age 14 was associated with steeper increases in substance use from 14–17 regardless of executive function levels. This finding replicates prior research that identified positive between-person associations between sensation seeking and substance use during adolescence and is in line with the externalizing pathway to substance use (Colder et al.,
2013). However, these findings may not provide direct support for the externalizing pathway, given the focus on sensation seeking and the lack of direct measurement of externalizing behaviors. At the within-person level, there were no associations between substance use and sensation seeking. Furthermore, there was no evidence that executive function moderated the sensation seeking-substance use association at the between-person level indicating that high executive function does not necessarily serve as a protective factor against substance use development for adolescents with high sensation seeking. This is consistent with prior research suggesting that sensation seeking and self-control have independent, not interactive, effects on risk taking (Duell et al.,
2016). These findings offer important insights into understanding the etiology of substance use during adolescence and identifying youth and adolescents who may benefit the most from substance use prevention programs. That is, those who show high sensation seeking during early adolescence are at the highest risk of developing substance use problems, regardless of executive function level. Conversely, there is no evidence indicating initial substance use as a risk factor for sensation seeking development. These findings have important intervention implications by suggesting that substance use may be prevented via early interventions targeting at-risk adolescents with high sensation seeking. Indeed, prior work has shown that personality-targeted interventions are effective in reducing substance use during adolescence in school-based settings (i.e., Conrod,
2016) and community-based settings (i.e., Edalati & Conrod,
2019). Given that the findings from the present study suggest the association between sensation seeking and substance use during adolescence is not moderated by executive function, interventions targeting executive function may not be most effective for youth high in sensation seeking. Instead, youth with high sensation seeking may benefit more from substance use prevention interventions such as motivational interviewing, which effectively reduces substance use by targeting reward sensitivity and motivation for change (Barnett et al.,
2012). Given that adolescent sensation seeking can promote healthy adjustment in certain contexts (Duell & Steinberg,
2020), in addition to reducing high levels of sensation seeking, interventions should aim to redirect sensation seeking from potentially detrimental risk taking (e.g., substance use) to positive risk taking (e.g., motivation for social justice). The present study points to the importance of sensation seeking for substance use particularly at the between-person level during adolescence, which can help inform the refinement of interventions to effectively reduce adolescent substance use.
The slope of sensation seeking was negatively associated with the slope of substance use for the low executive function group only. This may be interpreted in light of the direction of change in sensation seeking growth parameters for each group, and with caution given that the difference in the magnitude of this effect was not statistically significant between the two executive function groups. The high executive function group did not show significant change in the sensation seeking trajectory, whereas the low executive function group showed significant decreases in sensation seeking. Both groups showed significant increases in substance use. These data indicate that faster decreases in sensation seeking are related to slower increases in substance use among adolescents with low executive function, suggesting that those with low executive function would benefit from reducing sensation seeking concerning substance use progression during adolescence.
The present study has several limitations. First, despite the longitudinal study design, causality cannot be inferred due to the correlational nature of the data. Second, measures of negative affect, sensation seeking, and substance use were self-reported by adolescents, which could have introduced method variance and response bias. However, adolescents have been shown to be reliable reporters of their own internal states and sensation seeking behaviors (Huebner & Dew,
1995) and self-report measures are particularly revealing for behaviors that are related to internal experiences (Kendall et al.,
1989). Nevertheless, future work should consider utilizing multiple methods (e.g., neurobiological markers, behavioral performance, observations, other informants) to measure those Addictions Neuroclinical Assessment domains to gain a more comprehensive understanding of pathways leading to problematic substance use and addiction. Relatedly, negative affect was assessed annually, yet can be highly labile across days and moments (Naim et al.,
2022). Thus, future work may consider this process on a shorter timescale (i.e., across weeks or months) for a more comprehensive understanding of the role of negative affect in substance use. Third, executive function was dichotomized into two groups (i.e., low versus high) for the moderation analysis, and there are concerns regarding dichotomizing continuous variables due to simplifying variance (e.g., MacCallum et al.,
2002). We chose the multiple group structural equation modeling approach (1) to decrease model complexity given sample size, and (2) because it allowed us to systematically test where moderating effects of executive function were significant by imposing equality constraints one path at a time and examining changes in model fit using indices that were not influenced by sample size. Supplemental sensitivity analyses suggest that individuals at the extreme low-end of executive function may be particularly at-risk, but that associations between negative affect/sensation seeking, and substance use vary based on how executive function is grouped. Thus, future research with substantially larger sample sizes and more time points should consider examining interaction effects using executive function as a continuous variable to better understand how executive function moderates these associations at varying levels and test whether its moderating effects may change with development. Lastly, for the negative affect and sensation seeking measures, reliability estimates were somewhat low. However, low reliability leads to underestimates of effects, meaning the effects found may be stronger if the negative affect and sensation seeking measures had better reliability (Furr & Bacharach,
2008).
Despite these limitations, this investigation addressed several gaps in the literature. First, the present study included annual assessments of negative affect, sensation seeking, and substance use across adolescence. This is a critical developmental period for substance use initiation and neurobiological development in brain regions involved in reward/incentive sensitivity, emotional reactivity, and executive function. Examining developmental changes in negative affect, sensation seeking, and substance use during this time period allows for a deeper understanding of how the processes that may lead to substance use progression unfold. Second, the socioeconomically diverse sample includes adolescents in rural, suburban and urban settings from understudied and underserved communities that include multiple economically distressed counties, towns, and cities. These regions have current and historically high rates of substance use and addiction in comparison with the rest of the nation (Appalachian Regional Commission,
2023). Understanding the effects of biobehavioral markers of addiction on prospective development of substance use in this sample advances the theoretical understanding of risk and protective factors on substance use development among at-risk youth. Third, the present study utilized a multivariate LCM-SR, which facilitated the ability to disaggregate between-person and within-person processes by which negative affect, sensation seeking, and substance use change over time. Examining changes within individuals across time (i.e., how an individual moves from one state in time to the next on one dimension, depending on their state on another dimension) is directly relevant to intervention efforts. Understanding within-person changes helps identify which psychological domains to target and which can engender subsequent changes in symptoms. Further, examining the between-person effects while accounting for within-person effects clarifies the between-person differences in negative affect and sensation seeking related to substance use, and informs who may be at elevated risk for substance use progression, enabling targeted preventive intervention.