Skip to main content

Welkom bij Erasmus MC & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Erasmus MC heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van Erasmus MC, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van Erasmus MC.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top

23-11-2024 | Original Article

The Effects of Hearing One’s Own Name on Subsequent Attention to Visual Stimuli in Autistic and Neurotypical Children: An ERP Study

Auteurs: Yige Wang, Yong Liu, Xinling Wang, Keith M Kendrick, Tingyong Feng

Gepubliceerd in: Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Purpose

Hearing one’s own name produces unique patterns of brain activation which triggers attention and orienting responses to the caller. Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) rarely orientate towards people calling their own name, but the extent to which it may facilitate processing of the following external stimuli are not yet clear.

Methods

The current study consisted of both auditory and visual stimuli. Electroencephalogram (EEG) was measured in 28 autistic and neurotypical children (aged 3–7 years) to investigate auditory event-related brain potentials (ERPs) while hearing either their own or an unfamiliar name, and subsequent visual ERPs when viewing objects after hearing them.

Results

The results demonstrated that, unlike neurotypical children, autistic children did not show enhanced P300 responses upon hearing their own name, but exhibited more negative N1 response in the left frontal region to hearing their own name than an unfamiliar name. However, both autistic and neurotypical children showed equivalent changes in N2, P3 and Late positive potential (LPP) visual ERPs when viewing objects after hearing their own name relative to an unfamiliar name.

Conclusion

These findings suggest that autistic children who do not overtly respond to their own name (characterized by a head-turn), nevertheless exhibit increased attention to visual objects in their environment after hearing it. This implies that autistic children do recognize the sound of their name as important but may not understand the social meaning of it.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Alexopoulos, T., Muller, D., Ric, F., & Marendaz, C. (2012). I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 770–779.CrossRef Alexopoulos, T., Muller, D., Ric, F., & Marendaz, C. (2012). I, me, mine: Automatic attentional capture by self-related stimuli. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 770–779.CrossRef
go back to reference Bathelt, J., Dale, N., & de Haan, M. (2017). Event-related potential response to auditory social stimuli, parent-reported social communicative deficits and autism risk in school-aged children with congenital visual impairment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 10–18.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Bathelt, J., Dale, N., & de Haan, M. (2017). Event-related potential response to auditory social stimuli, parent-reported social communicative deficits and autism risk in school-aged children with congenital visual impairment. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 27, 10–18.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16(4), 298–304.PubMedCrossRef Bortfeld, H., Morgan, J. L., Golinkoff, R. M., & Rathbun, K. (2005). Mommy and me: Familiar names help launch babies into speech-stream segmentation. Psychological Science, 16(4), 298–304.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chen, J., Yuan, J., Feng, T., Chen, A., Gu, B., & Li, H. (2011). Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 290–295.PubMedCrossRef Chen, J., Yuan, J., Feng, T., Chen, A., Gu, B., & Li, H. (2011). Temporal features of the degree effect in self-relevance: Neural correlates. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 290–295.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 79–93.PubMedCrossRef Chita-Tegmark, M. (2016). Social attention in ASD: A review and meta-analysis of eye-tracking studies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 48, 79–93.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conine, D. E., Vollmer, T. R., & Bolívar, H. A. (2020). Response to name in children with autism: Treatment, generalization, and maintenance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 744–766.PubMedCrossRef Conine, D. E., Vollmer, T. R., & Bolívar, H. A. (2020). Response to name in children with autism: Treatment, generalization, and maintenance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 53(2), 744–766.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Conty, L., N’Diaye, K., Tijus, C., & George, N. (2007). When eye creates the contact! ERP evidence for early dissociation between direct and averted gaze motion processing. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3024–3037.PubMedCrossRef Conty, L., N’Diaye, K., Tijus, C., & George, N. (2007). When eye creates the contact! ERP evidence for early dissociation between direct and averted gaze motion processing. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3024–3037.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Cook, J. L., Rapp, J. T., Mann, K. R., McHugh, C., Burji, C., & Nuta, R. (2017). A practitioner model for increasing eye contact in children with autism. Behavior Modification, 41, 382–404.PubMedCrossRef Cook, J. L., Rapp, J. T., Mann, K. R., McHugh, C., Burji, C., & Nuta, R. (2017). A practitioner model for increasing eye contact in children with autism. Behavior Modification, 41, 382–404.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, A. M. (1981a). Event- related brain potentials to human faces in infants. Child Development, 52, 804–811.PubMedCrossRef Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, A. M. (1981a). Event- related brain potentials to human faces in infants. Child Development, 52, 804–811.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, A. M. (1981b). Event-related brain potentials to human faces in infants. Child Development, 52, 804–811.PubMedCrossRef Courchesne, E., Ganz, L., & Norcia, A. M. (1981b). Event-related brain potentials to human faces in infants. Child Development, 52, 804–811.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–168.CrossRef Csibra, G. (2010). Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language, 25(2), 141–168.CrossRef
go back to reference Cygan, H. B., Tacikowski, P., Ostaszewski, P., Chojnicka, I., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates of own name and own face detection in autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86020.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Cygan, H. B., Tacikowski, P., Ostaszewski, P., Chojnicka, I., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates of own name and own face detection in autism spectrum disorder. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86020.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.PubMedCrossRef Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271–283.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.PubMedCrossRef Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 134(1), 9–21.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Dietrich, D. E., Waller, C., Johannes, S., Wieringa, B. M., Emrich, H., & Münte, T. (2001). Differential effects of emotional content on event-related potentials in word recognition memory. Neuropsychobiology, 43(2), 96.PubMedCrossRef Dietrich, D. E., Waller, C., Johannes, S., Wieringa, B. M., Emrich, H., & Münte, T. (2001). Differential effects of emotional content on event-related potentials in word recognition memory. Neuropsychobiology, 43(2), 96.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Eichenlaub, J. B., Ruby, P., & Morlet, D. (2012). What is the specificity of the response to the own first-name when presented as a novel in a passive oddball paradigm? An ERP study. Brain Research, 1447, 65–78.PubMedCrossRef Eichenlaub, J. B., Ruby, P., & Morlet, D. (2012). What is the specificity of the response to the own first-name when presented as a novel in a passive oddball paradigm? An ERP study. Brain Research, 1447, 65–78.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Fields, E. C., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). It’s all about you: An ERP study of emotion and self-relevance in discourse. NeuroImage, 62(1), 562–574.PubMedCrossRef Fields, E. C., & Kuperberg, G. R. (2012). It’s all about you: An ERP study of emotion and self-relevance in discourse. NeuroImage, 62(1), 562–574.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grainger, C., Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2014). Online action monitoring and memory for self-performed actions in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 1193–1206.PubMedCrossRef Grainger, C., Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2014). Online action monitoring and memory for self-performed actions in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 1193–1206.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Grisdale, E., Lind, S. E., Eacott, M. J., & Williams, D. M. (2014). Self-referential memory in autism spectrum disorder and typical development: Exploring the ownership effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 133–141.PubMedCrossRef Grisdale, E., Lind, S. E., Eacott, M. J., & Williams, D. M. (2014). Self-referential memory in autism spectrum disorder and typical development: Exploring the ownership effect. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 133–141.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Hartley, C., & Fisher, S. (2018). Mine is better than yours: Investigating the ownership effect in children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children. Cognition, 172, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef Hartley, C., & Fisher, S. (2018). Mine is better than yours: Investigating the ownership effect in children with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing children. Cognition, 172, 26–36.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Henderson, H. A., Zahka, N. E., Kojkowski, N. M., Inge, A. P., Schwartz, C. B., Hileman, C. M., & Mundy, P. C. (2009). Self-referenced memory, social cognition, and symptom presentation in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(7), 853–861.PubMedCrossRef Henderson, H. A., Zahka, N. E., Kojkowski, N. M., Inge, A. P., Schwartz, C. B., Hileman, C. M., & Mundy, P. C. (2009). Self-referenced memory, social cognition, and symptom presentation in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(7), 853–861.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Holeckova, I., Fischer, C., Giard, M. H., Delpuech, C., & Morlet, D. (2006). Brain responses to a subject’s own name uttered by a familiar voice. Brain Research, 1082(1), 142–152.PubMedCrossRef Holeckova, I., Fischer, C., Giard, M. H., Delpuech, C., & Morlet, D. (2006). Brain responses to a subject’s own name uttered by a familiar voice. Brain Research, 1082(1), 142–152.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2016). Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN). Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1–4), 5–17.PubMedCrossRef Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2016). Attentional control and the self: The Self-Attention Network (SAN). Cognitive Neuroscience, 7(1–4), 5–17.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Imafuku, M., Hakuno, Y., Uchida-Ota, M., Yamamoto, J. I., & Minagawa, Y. (2014). “Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers. NeuroImage, 103, 476–484.PubMedCrossRef Imafuku, M., Hakuno, Y., Uchida-Ota, M., Yamamoto, J. I., & Minagawa, Y. (2014). “Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers. NeuroImage, 103, 476–484.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Jolicoeur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2006). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, 70(6), 414–424.PubMedCrossRef Jolicoeur, P., Sessa, P., Dell’Acqua, R., & Robitaille, N. (2006). On the control of visual spatial attention: Evidence from human electrophysiology. Psychological Research, 70(6), 414–424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Kampe, K. K. W., Frithl, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “Mentalizing”, regardless of modality. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 5258–5263.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Kampe, K. K. W., Frithl, C. D., & Frith, U. (2003). “Hey John”: Signals conveying communicative intention toward the self activate brain regions associated with “Mentalizing”, regardless of modality. The Journal of Neuroscience, 23(12), 5258–5263.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Lind, S. E. (2010). Memory and the self in autism: A review and theoretical framework. Autism, 14(5), 430–456.PubMedCrossRef Lind, S. E. (2010). Memory and the self in autism: A review and theoretical framework. Autism, 14(5), 430–456.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., Sadek, S. A., Pasco, G., Wheelwright, S. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain, 133(Pt 2), 611–624.PubMedCrossRef Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., Sadek, S. A., Pasco, G., Wheelwright, S. J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2010). Atypical neural self-representation in autism. Brain, 133(Pt 2), 611–624.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Pisoni, D. B. (1995). Infants’ recognition of the sound patterns of their own names. Psychological Science, 6(5), 314–317.PubMedCrossRef Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Pisoni, D. B. (1995). Infants’ recognition of the sound patterns of their own names. Psychological Science, 6(5), 314–317.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Mandel-Emer, D., Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D., Seidl, A., Hollich, G., Johnson, E., & Jusczyk, A. (2003). What’s in a Name?: How Infants Respond to Some Familiar Sound Patterns. Psychological Science, 314 - 317. Mandel-Emer, D., Jusczyk, P. W., Houston, D., Seidl, A., Hollich, G., Johnson, E., & Jusczyk, A. (2003). What’s in a Name?: How Infants Respond to Some Familiar Sound Patterns. Psychological Science, 314 - 317.
go back to reference Miller, M., Iosif, A. M., Hill, M., Young, G. S., Schwichtenberg, A. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2017). Response to name in infants developing autism spectrum disorder: A prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 183, 141-146.e1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Miller, M., Iosif, A. M., Hill, M., Young, G. S., Schwichtenberg, A. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2017). Response to name in infants developing autism spectrum disorder: A prospective study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 183, 141-146.e1.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Muñoz, F., Casado, P., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., Jiménez-Ortega, L., Fondevila, S., Espuny, J., Sánchez-García, J., & Martín-Loeches, M. (2019). Neural dynamics in the processing of personal objects as an index of the brain representation of the self. Brain Topography, 33(1), 86–100.PubMedCrossRef Muñoz, F., Casado, P., Hernández-Gutiérrez, D., Jiménez-Ortega, L., Fondevila, S., Espuny, J., Sánchez-García, J., & Martín-Loeches, M. (2019). Neural dynamics in the processing of personal objects as an index of the brain representation of the self. Brain Topography, 33(1), 86–100.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Muñoz, V., Muñoz-Caracuel, M., Angulo-Ruiz, B. Y., & Gómez, C. M. (2023). Neurovascular coupling during auditory stimulation: Event-related potentials and fNIRS hemodynamic. Brain Structure & Function, 228(8), 1943–1961.CrossRef Muñoz, V., Muñoz-Caracuel, M., Angulo-Ruiz, B. Y., & Gómez, C. M. (2023). Neurovascular coupling during auditory stimulation: Event-related potentials and fNIRS hemodynamic. Brain Structure & Function, 228(8), 1943–1961.CrossRef
go back to reference Nadig, A. S., Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 378–383.PubMedCrossRef Nadig, A. S., Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Rozga, A., Sigman, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2007). A prospective study of response to name in infants at risk for autism. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 378–383.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Newman, R. S. (2005). The cocktail party effect in infants revisited: Listening to one’s name in noise. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 352–362.PubMedCrossRef Newman, R. S. (2005). The cocktail party effect in infants revisited: Listening to one’s name in noise. Developmental Psychology, 41(2), 352–362.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nijhof, A. D., Dhar, M., Goris, J., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2018). Atypical neural responding to hearing one’s own name in adults with ASD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(1), 129–138.PubMedCrossRef Nijhof, A. D., Dhar, M., Goris, J., Brass, M., & Wiersema, J. R. (2018). Atypical neural responding to hearing one’s own name in adults with ASD. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(1), 129–138.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Nijhof, A. D., von Trott Zu Solz, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2021). Equivalent own name bias in autism: An EEG study of the Attentional Blink. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. Nijhof, A. D., von Trott Zu Solz, J., Catmur, C., & Bird, G. (2021). Equivalent own name bias in autism: An EEG study of the Attentional Blink. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci.
go back to reference Niu, G., Yao, L., Kong, F., Luo, Y., Duan, C., Sun, X., & Zhou, Z. (2020). Behavioural and ERP evidence of the self-advantage of online self-relevant information. Science and Reports, 10(1), 20515.CrossRef Niu, G., Yao, L., Kong, F., Luo, Y., Duan, C., Sun, X., & Zhou, Z. (2020). Behavioural and ERP evidence of the self-advantage of online self-relevant information. Science and Reports, 10(1), 20515.CrossRef
go back to reference Nowicka, A., Cygan, H. B., Tacikowski, P., Ostaszewski, P., & Kus, R. (2016). Name recognition in autism: EEG evidence of altered patterns of brain activity and connectivity. Mol Autism, 7(1), 38.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Nowicka, A., Cygan, H. B., Tacikowski, P., Ostaszewski, P., & Kus, R. (2016). Name recognition in autism: EEG evidence of altered patterns of brain activity and connectivity. Mol Autism, 7(1), 38.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Parise, E., Friederici, A. D., & Striano, T. (2010). “Did you call me?” 5-month-old infants own name guides their attention. PLoS ONE, 5(12), e14208.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Parise, E., Friederici, A. D., & Striano, T. (2010). “Did you call me?” 5-month-old infants own name guides their attention. PLoS ONE, 5(12), e14208.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Parmentier, F. B., Elsley, J. V., & Ljungberg, J. K. (2010). Behavioral distraction by auditory novelty is not only about novelty: The role of the distracter’s informational value. Cognition, 115(3), 504–511.PubMedCrossRef Parmentier, F. B., Elsley, J. V., & Ljungberg, J. K. (2010). Behavioral distraction by auditory novelty is not only about novelty: The role of the distracter’s informational value. Cognition, 115(3), 504–511.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Perrin, F., Garcı́a-Larrea, L., Mauguière, F., & Bastuji, H. (1999). A differential brain response to the subject’s own name persists during sleep. Clinical Neurophysiology, 12(110), 2153–2164.CrossRef Perrin, F., Garcı́a-Larrea, L., Mauguière, F., & Bastuji, H. (1999). A differential brain response to the subject’s own name persists during sleep. Clinical Neurophysiology, 12(110), 2153–2164.CrossRef
go back to reference Portas, C. M., Krakow, K., Allen, P., Josephs, O., Armony, J. L., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Auditory processing across the sleep-wake cycle: Simultaneous EEG and fMRI monitoring in humans. Neuron, 28(3), 991–999.PubMedCrossRef Portas, C. M., Krakow, K., Allen, P., Josephs, O., Armony, J. L., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Auditory processing across the sleep-wake cycle: Simultaneous EEG and fMRI monitoring in humans. Neuron, 28(3), 991–999.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference SanMiguel, I., Linden, D., & Escera, C. (2010). Attention capture by novel sounds: Distraction versus facilitation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(4), 481–515.CrossRef SanMiguel, I., Linden, D., & Escera, C. (2010). Attention capture by novel sounds: Distraction versus facilitation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(4), 481–515.CrossRef
go back to reference Schuller, A. M., & Rossion, B. (2004). Perception of static eye gaze direction facilitates subsequent early visual processing. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(5), 1161–1168.PubMedCrossRef Schuller, A. M., & Rossion, B. (2004). Perception of static eye gaze direction facilitates subsequent early visual processing. Clinical Neurophysiology, 115(5), 1161–1168.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., & Sorensen, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 23(2), 504–514.PubMedCrossRef Shapiro, K. L., Caldwell, J., & Sorensen, R. E. (1997). Personal names and the attentional blink: A visual “cocktail party” effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance, 23(2), 504–514.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tacikowski, P., Jednorog, K., Marchewka, A., & Nowicka, A. (2011). How multiple repetitions influence the processing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 219–230.PubMedCrossRef Tacikowski, P., Jednorog, K., Marchewka, A., & Nowicka, A. (2011). How multiple repetitions influence the processing of self-, famous and unknown names and faces: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 219–230.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Tacikowski, P., Cygan, H. B., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates of own and close-other’s name recognition: ERP evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 194.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Tacikowski, P., Cygan, H. B., & Nowicka, A. (2014). Neural correlates of own and close-other’s name recognition: ERP evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 194.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Tateuchi, T., Itoh, K., & Nakada, T. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the orienting response to subject’s own name: An event-related potential study. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 786–791.PubMedCrossRef Tateuchi, T., Itoh, K., & Nakada, T. (2012). Neural mechanisms underlying the orienting response to subject’s own name: An event-related potential study. Psychophysiology, 49(6), 786–791.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Thomas, R. P., Wang, L. A. L., Guthrie, W., Cola, M., McCleery, J. P., Pandey, J., & Miller, J. S. (2019). What’s in a name? A preliminary event-related potential study of response to name in preschool children with and without autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One, 14(5), e0216051.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef Thomas, R. P., Wang, L. A. L., Guthrie, W., Cola, M., McCleery, J. P., Pandey, J., & Miller, J. S. (2019). What’s in a name? A preliminary event-related potential study of response to name in preschool children with and without autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One, 14(5), e0216051.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRef
go back to reference Toichi, M., Kamio, Y., Okada, T., Sakihama, M., Youngstrom, E. A., Findling, R. L., & Yamamoto, K. (2002). A lack of self-consciousness in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(8), 1422–1424.PubMedCrossRef Toichi, M., Kamio, Y., Okada, T., Sakihama, M., Youngstrom, E. A., Findling, R. L., & Yamamoto, K. (2002). A lack of self-consciousness in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(8), 1422–1424.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3657–3668.PubMedCrossRef Turk, D. J., van Bussel, K., Waiter, G., & Macrae, C. N. (2011). Mine and me: Exploring the neural basis of object ownership. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3657–3668.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Webb, S. J., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Namkung, J., Toth, K., Greenson, J., Murias, M., & Dawson, G. (2010). Toddlers with elevated autism symptoms show slowed habituation to faces. Child Neuropsychology : A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 16(3), 255–278.PubMedCrossRef Webb, S. J., Jones, E. J., Merkle, K., Namkung, J., Toth, K., Greenson, J., Murias, M., & Dawson, G. (2010). Toddlers with elevated autism symptoms show slowed habituation to faces. Child Neuropsychology : A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 16(3), 255–278.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Webb, S. J., Neuhaus, E., & Faja, S. (2017). Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 70(5), 970–986.PubMedCrossRef Webb, S. J., Neuhaus, E., & Faja, S. (2017). Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 70(5), 970–986.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief Report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Joural of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157.CrossRef Werner, E., Dawson, G., Osterling, J., & Dinno, N. (2000). Brief Report: Recognition of autism spectrum disorder before one year of age: A retrospective study based on home videotapes. Joural of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 157.CrossRef
go back to reference Williams, D. (2010). Theory of own mind in autism: Evidence of a specific deficit in self-awareness? Autism, 14(5), 474–494.PubMedCrossRef Williams, D. (2010). Theory of own mind in autism: Evidence of a specific deficit in self-awareness? Autism, 14(5), 474–494.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Williams, D. M., Nicholson, T., & Grainger, C. (2018). The self-reference effect on perception: Undiminished in adults with autism and no relation to autism traits. Autism Research, 11(2), 331–341.PubMedCrossRef Williams, D. M., Nicholson, T., & Grainger, C. (2018). The self-reference effect on perception: Undiminished in adults with autism and no relation to autism traits. Autism Research, 11(2), 331–341.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Wood, N. L., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: Attention and memory in the classic selective listening procedure of Cherry (1953). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 255–260. Wood, N. L., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: Attention and memory in the classic selective listening procedure of Cherry (1953). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 255–260.
go back to reference Zhang, Q., Luo, C., Ngetich, R., Zhang, J., Jin, Z., & Li, L. (2022). Visual selective attention P300 source in frontal-parietal lobe: ERP and fMRI study. Brain Topography, 35(5–6), 636–650.PubMedCrossRef Zhang, Q., Luo, C., Ngetich, R., Zhang, J., Jin, Z., & Li, L. (2022). Visual selective attention P300 source in frontal-parietal lobe: ERP and fMRI study. Brain Topography, 35(5–6), 636–650.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zhao, S., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2018). A functional but atypical self: Influence of self-relevant processing on the gaze cueing effect in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 11(11), 1522–1531.PubMedCrossRef Zhao, S., Uono, S., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2018). A functional but atypical self: Influence of self-relevant processing on the gaze cueing effect in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, 11(11), 1522–1531.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zhong, J., Liu, Y., Zhang, E., Luo, J., & Chen, J. (2013). Individuals’ attentional bias toward an envied target’s name: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 550, 109–114.PubMedCrossRef Zhong, J., Liu, Y., Zhang, E., Luo, J., & Chen, J. (2013). Individuals’ attentional bias toward an envied target’s name: An event-related potential study. Neuroscience Letters, 550, 109–114.PubMedCrossRef
go back to reference Zhu, S., Long, Q., Li, X., Yang, J., Li, H., & Yuan, J. (2018). Self-relevant processing of stranger’s name in Chinese society: Surname matters. Neuroscience Letters, 668, 126–132.PubMedCrossRef Zhu, S., Long, Q., Li, X., Yang, J., Li, H., & Yuan, J. (2018). Self-relevant processing of stranger’s name in Chinese society: Surname matters. Neuroscience Letters, 668, 126–132.PubMedCrossRef
Metagegevens
Titel
The Effects of Hearing One’s Own Name on Subsequent Attention to Visual Stimuli in Autistic and Neurotypical Children: An ERP Study
Auteurs
Yige Wang
Yong Liu
Xinling Wang
Keith M Kendrick
Tingyong Feng
Publicatiedatum
23-11-2024
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
Print ISSN: 0162-3257
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-3432
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-024-06639-1