Erasmus MC heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.
Om ook buiten de locaties van Erasmus MC, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van Erasmus MC.
Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.
Login
Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.
Of all psychiatric disorders, the disruptive behavior disorders (DBDs) are the most likely to predispose to substance dependence (SD). One possible underlying mechanism for this increased vulnerability is risky decision making. The aim of this study was to examine decision making in DBD adolescents with and without SD. Twenty-five DBD adolescents (19 males) with SD (DBD+SD), 28 DBD adolescents (23 males) without SD (DBD-SD) and 99 healthy controls (72 males) were included in the study. DBD adolescents with co-morbid attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were excluded. Risky decision making was investigated by assessing the number of disadvantageous choices in the Iowa gambling task. DBD+SD made significantly more risky choices than healthy controls and DBD-SD. Healthy controls and DBD-SD did not differ on risky decision making. These results suggest that risky decision making is a vulnerability factor for the development of SD in a subgroup of adolescents with DBD without ADHD.
Of all psychiatric disorders, the Disruptive Behavior Disorders (DBDs), i.e., Conduct Disorder (CD) and Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD), are the most likely to predispose to substance dependence (SD) (Costello et al. 2003; Disney et al. 1999; Fergusson et al. 2007; Kim-Cohen et al. 2003; Merikangas and Avenevoli 2000; Rutter et al. 2006; Young et al. 1995). The mechanisms underlying this increased vulnerability in the DBDs, however, remain poorly understood. Behavioral genetic research has shown that adolescent CD, adult antisocial behavior and SD share a highly heritable general vulnerability (Hicks et al. 2004). This shared genetic vulnerability may be manifested already at an early age in the form of abnormalities in the sensitivity for reward and punishment as well as deficiencies in the ability to balance immediate rewards against long-term negative consequences.
In support of this notion, response perseveration, i.e., the tendency to continue a previously rewarded response that is now punished, has been repeatedly demonstrated in children and adolescents with DBDs (Daugherty and Quay 1991; Matthys et al. 1998; Shapiro et al. 1988; Van Goozen et al. 2004), in children with psychopathic tendencies (O’Brien and Frick 1996), and in young non-alcoholic men with a multigenerational family history of alcoholism (Giancola et al. 1993). Perseverative behavior may contribute to impairments in decision making by rendering individuals unable to shift their behavior away from immediate rewards that are associated with even larger future punishments. This may be of relevance for SD, since continued drug use despite explicit knowledge of its negative consequences is a defining characteristic of SD (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Indeed, disturbances in the balance between short-term gains and long-term losses have been observed both in adults with SD and adults with antisocial disorders (Bartzokis et al. 2000; Bechara et al. 2001; Grant et al. 2000; Mazas et al. 2000).
Although a variety of studies have investigated decision making in children and adolescents with DBD, co-morbidity with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has not been taken into account. Impaired decision making has been found in DBD adolescents (Fairchild et al. 2009). However, in the Fairchild et al. (2009) study the sample consisted of adolescents with DBD with or without ADHD. It is important to take the co-morbidity of DBDs with ADHD into account, not only because impaired decision making has been found in adolescents with ADHD (Toplak et al. 2005), but also because children with ADHD are at risk for SD (Biederman et al. 2006). Likewise, in a recent study by Luman et al. (2010) deficits in decision making in children with ODD may have been caused by ADHD, as 16 of the 18 children with ODD included in this study also met criteria of ADHD. Finally, in the study on decision making by Ernst et al. (2003) the sample was heterogeneous, consisting of adolescents with ADHD without co-morbidity, adolescents with DBD without co-morbidity, and ADHD adolescents with a history of mood disorders but without acute symptoms. In sum, it remains unclear whether decision making deficits are associated with DBD, ADHD, or both.
To assess decision making in children and adolescents with DBD (Ernst et al. 2003; Fairchild et al. 2009; Luman et al. 2010) and in adults with SD (Bechara et al. 2001; Bechara et al. 2002) gambling tasks such as the Iowa gambling task (IGT) are often used. The IGT measures decision-making processes by simulating real-life decisions involving reward, punishment, and uncertainty of outcomes. During the IGT, healthy participants learn to favor immediate small over large rewards to avoid large future punishments. Interestingly, low punishment sensitivity has been associated with response perseveration in boys with DBD (Matthys et al. 2004). In addition, high reward sensitivity correlates with overall risky decision making, impulsivity and substance use (Blum et al. 2000; Giancola et al. 1993). Thus, differences in the balance between reward and punishment sensitivity, as well as perseverative behavior associated with DBD and SD may contribute to impaired decision making in these disorders.
Alternatively, animal studies have shown that exposure to drugs of abuse may cause impairments such as hypersensitivity to reward, hyposensitivity to punishment and perseverative behavior, that may contribute to disturbances in decision making (Deroche-Gamonet et al. 2004; Fletcher et al. 2005; Jentsch et al. 2002; Roesch et al. 2007; Schoenbaum et al. 2004; Schoenbaum and Setlow 2005; Vanderschuren and Everitt 2004). This suggests that risky decision making may not only be a cause, but could also be a consequence of SD.
The aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between decision making and SD in DBD adolescents. To that end, we compared risky decision making in DBD adolescents with (DBD+SD) and without SD (DBD-SD), and healthy controls. In order to exclude possible contributions of ADHD to decision making deficits in DBD, DBD adolescents with co-morbid ADHD were excluded from the study. We anticipated that if impaired decision making is associated with DBD, then DBD+SD and DBD-SD will demonstrate decision making deficits when compared with healthy controls. Alternatively, if impaired decision making is a vulnerability factor for SD in DBD then among adolescents with DBD only those with SD (DBD+SD) will demonstrate deficits in decision making.
Methods
Participants
Fifty-three adolescents (mean±SD age, 17.2 ± 1.7 years) meeting the criteria for DBD, that is either CD or ODD in accordance with the DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000) were recruited from judicial and forensic/orthopsychiatric institutions for youth in the Netherlands, and compared to 99 adolescents in a healthy control group (HC). ODD was included because institutionalization precludes the occurrence of some behaviors or symptoms that would normally result in a CD diagnosis. The HC group (mean±SD age, 17.5 ± 1.9 years) consisted of a selected sample from an existing database of participants (Zonnevylle-Bender et al. 2007) and newly recruited volunteers from vocational training schools in the Netherlands. None of the DBD or HC adolescents had a DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of ADHD or schizophrenia. SD was diagnosed according to DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association 2000). Psychiatric diagnoses of CD, ODD, ADHD and SD were obtained using the NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children-Child Version (DISC-IV-C) (Shaffer et al. 2000). The NIMH DISC-IV-C interview was performed by a trained psychologist. The proportion of CD/ODD diagnoses in the DBD-SD (24/4) and the DBD+SD (21/5) was equal, χ2 = 0.03: p = 0.86. None of the HC included in the present sample had a diagnosis of SD. General intellectual functioning was estimated with the averaged score on the ‘Vocabulary’ and ‘Block designs’ subtests of either the Dutch version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, third edition (WISC-III-NL) or the Dutch version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, third edition (Vander Steene et al. 1986; Wechsler 1997). In children, these subtests have a correlation of 0.90 with the full-scale IQ (Sattler 1992). Details on the main characteristics of the DBD and HC adolescents group are shown in Table 1. In the DBD+SD group 28% of the participants was dependent on alcohol, 48% on nicotine, 76% on marihuana and 24% of the participants used other drugs.
Table 1
Main characteristics of the DBD with (DBD+SD) and without substance dependence (DBD-SD) and healthy control (HC) adolescents group
HC
DBD-SD
DBD+SD
p-value
(n = 99)
(n = 28)
(n = 25)
Age, mean±SD, years
17.5 ± 1.9
16.8 ± 1.5
17.5 ± 1.8
0.18i
Intelligence, mean±SD
88.8 ± 19.2
78.7 ± 12.3
82.3 ± 15.2
0.02i
Male/female, N
72/27
23/5
19/6
<0.01ii
iAnalysis of variance
iiχ2 test
Iowa Gambling Task
A computerized version of the IGT was used to examine risky decision making (Bechara et al. 1994). Participants were instructed to win as much fictitious money as possible by picking one card at a time from any of the four decks (A, B, C, and D) until the instruction was given to stop. Unbeknownst to the players, the task ends after 100 card selections. The four decks (40 cards each) vary with respect to immediate gains and risk of penalties. Selections from decks A and B yield average immediate gains of € 100, whereas decks C and D on average give € 50 for every card. In addition, each deck also carries certain penalties, such that in the long run the amount of penalties exceed the amount of gains in decks A and B, and the amount of penalties are smaller than the amount of gains in decks C and D. The optimal strategy is therefore to minimize the overall loss by avoiding the disadvantageous decks A and B in favor of advantageous decks C and D. Since the aim of the task is to win as much money as possible, decisions to choose from the advantageous decks C and D should therefore increase over time as a result of the reward and punishment schedules inherent to the task (Bechara et al. 1994).
Procedure
Testing took place at the institution where the participant stayed or at home. All participants were administered the same test-battery, consisting of the NIMH DISC-IV-C, WISC-III-NL or WAIS-III and the IGT. Cases where the participants reported they had used alcohol, cannabis or other drugs during the 24 h prior to testing were excluded from the study. Testing took approximately 4 h per participant. All volunteers received written and verbal information on the study protocol. Written informed consent was obtained from the participants and also parental assent in case the participant was younger than 18 years of age. Subjects received a 25 Euro gift certificate for participation. The study was approved by the Medical Ethical Review Committee of the University Medical Center Utrecht in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki.
Data Reduction and Analyses
Risky decision making was measured by dividing the 100 card selections into five blocks of 20 card selections (Bechara et al. 1994) and calculated by computing the mean percentage choices from the disadvantageous decks A and B for each block. Risky decision making was examined by performing an ANOVA over the five blocks with group [HC, DBD-SD, DBD+SD] as the between-subjects factor. Age and intelligence were entered as covariates (Blair et al. 2001; Evans et al. 2004). A separate ANOVA over the five blocks was performed to examine possible sex differences between the three groups [HC, DBD+SD, DBD-SD] in decision making. Total monetary outcome among the groups was investigated by running an ANOVA with group [HC, DBD+SD, DBD-SD] as the between subjects factor and age and intelligence as covariates. Where appropriate, a Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used. The significance level was set at 0.05, two-tailed.
Results
Risky decision making was significantly influenced by age, F(4, 148) = 3.35, p = 0.01, and intelligence, F(4, 148) = 2.9, p = 0.02. Older children and higher IQ scores were associated with reduced risky decision making. The test of between-subjects effects controlled for age and intelligence demonstrated a main effect of group, F(2, 150) = 3.14, p < 0.05. No main effect of sex, F(1, 151) = 0.01, p = 0.96, and no group x sex interaction, F(2, 150) = 1.13, p = 0.32, was found, suggesting that the significant main effect of group on risky decision making could not be explained by differences in male–female distribution across the three groups. Post-hoc least square difference (LSD) analyses controlling for age and intelligence showed that DBD+SD overall made more risky decisions than HC (p = 0.05), and DBD-SD (p = 0.02), whereas no difference was observed between HC and DBD-SD (p = 0.31). Figure 1 displays overall percentage risky decision making for the DBD+SD (n = 25), DBD-SD (n = 28) and HC group (n = 99). The ANOVA corrected for age and intelligence demonstrated a significant main effect of block, F(4, 148) = 5.44, p < 0.01, ε = 0.75, but no group x block interaction, F(8, 144) = 0.44, p = 0.89, ε = 0.75. Percentage risky decision making across the five blocks of the three groups is presented in Fig. 2. No group differences on monetary outcome were observed, F(2, 150) = 2.33, p = 0.1.
Fig. 1
Age- and intelligence-corrected mean±standard-error of the grand average percentage risk taking (i.e., total choices of deck A and B / 100) of the IGT in healthy controls (HC) and DBD adolescents without (DBD-SD) and with SD (DBD+SD). Covariates appearing in the model are evaluated at age = 17.37 and intelligence = 85.89. *p = 0.05, **p = 0.02
Fig. 2
Age and intelligence corrected mean±standard-error of the percentage risk taking (i.e., choices of deck A and B / 20) for each block of the IGT in healthy controls (HC), and DBD adolescents without (DBD-SD) and with substance dependence (DBD+SD). Covariates appearing in the model are evaluated at age = 17.37 and intelligence = 85.89
×
×
Discussion
The aim of the present study was to examine the relationship between impaired decision making and SD in DBD adolescents. Results showed that DBD+SD adolescents demonstrated an overall preference for the risky disadvantageous decks in comparison to DBD-SD and HC adolescents, whereas DBD-SD did not differ from HC adolescents on risk taking. However, DBD adolescents with and without SD did not demonstrate differences in risky decision making across the blocks during the IGT. The present study also showed that intelligence and age contribute to IGT performance (Blair et al. 2001; Evans et al. 2004).
Results of the present study suggest that risky decision making is not a general characteristic of DBD adolescents without ADHD co-morbidity. This result is not necessarily at variance with earlier findings by Ernst et al. (2003), Fairchild et al. (2009), and Luman et al. (2010) as samples in these studies did not consist of DBD-only adolescents. Therefore, our results emphasize the importance of considering the co-morbidity between ADHD and DBD, in order to properly understand the cognitive characteristics of these disorders. Rather, instead of being a general characteristic of DBD, it may be that risky decision making, which has been found to be strongly associated with SD (Bartzokis et al. 2000; Bechara et al. 2001; Grant et al. 2000; Mazas et al. 2000), is a vulnerability factor for SD in a subgroup of adolescents with DBD. However, it is also possible that risky decision making is a reflection of the deleterious effects on behavior of excessive drug use in the DBD+SD.
Indeed, preclinical studies addressing the relationship between decision making and addictive behavior have shown that repeated or prolonged exposure to psychostimulant drugs such as amphetamine or cocaine increases the sensitivity for reward (Roesch et al. 2007), reduces the sensitivity to punishment signals during drug seeking (Deroche-Gamonet et al. 2004; Vanderschuren and Everitt 2004), and induces inflexible and perseverative behavior (Fletcher et al. 2005; Schoenbaum et al. 2004; Schoenbaum and Setlow 2005). Thus, drug-induced alterations in behavior may explain the enhanced risk taking we observed in DBD+SD.
On the other hand, severity of symptoms in terms of chronic dependence (>1 year) and range of chronically abused substances in our sample did not have an effect on the percentage risk taking (both p-values >0.27). These results suggest that in spite of our small group sizes, increased risk taking in DBD adolescents with SD does not seem to be strongly mediated by chronic dependence. This observation concurs with animal studies showing that highly impulsive rats more rapidly develop symptoms of addiction than their less impulsive counterparts (Belin et al. 2008; Diergaarde et al. 2008). The faster onset of symptoms related to SD can therefore be explained by the fact that impulsivity is associated with an imbalance between the reward and punishment systems (Potts et al. 2006; Smillie and Jackson 2006), supporting the notion that impaired decision making may precede and importantly contribute to the development of SD in DBD.
Together, these findings highlight the complex interactions between the vulnerability to SD on the one hand, and the detrimental effects of drug exposure on the other hand. The present results show that increased risk taking is associated with DBD+SD, underscoring the need for further research on the relationship between reward and punishment sensitivity, perseverative behavior, substance use and decision making in DBD (Rogers and Robbins 2001).
We wish to note that risky decision making by itself does not necessarily imply abnormal decision making or a psychopathological condition. A balance between acquired knowledge as reflected by the decline in risky decision making during the course of the game and the exploration of new options in terms of incidental risk taking has been suggested to be the optimal behavioral strategy to seek and exploit new opportunities to maximize profit (Daw et al. 2006). However, in order to maximize gains on the IGT, an optimal balance between exploration and exploitation is necessary, and the increases in risk taking as observed here in DBD+SD usually lead to reduced profits on the task. Unfortunately, in spite of increased risky decision making by DBD+SD adolescents, no group differences in monetary outcomes were observed. A possible explanation might be that the use of real instead of facsimile money may increase risky decision making (Bowman and Turnbull 2003). The fact that we used facsimile money in the present study may have prevented us from finding a significant group difference on monetary outcome. In other words, due to the hypothetical payments we cannot rule out the possibility that some participants may not have taken the task seriously. Finally, we controlled for the confounding effects of age, intelligence and ADHD diagnoses in our sample, subsequently increasing homogeneity of the group but at the expense of statistical power.
Decision making is one of the cognitive skills that individuals use to solve everyday social problems. Children and adolescents, indeed, have at their disposal a series of cognitive skills such as defining the problem, generating possible solutions and deciding which solution will be implemented (Crick and Dodge 1994). Deviances in all these skills have been found in children with aggressive behavior and DBD (Matthys and Lochman 2010). Specifically, with respect to the response decision step it has been shown that DBD children more often select an aggressive response and less often select a prosocial response when compared with normal controls (Matthys et al. 1999). Cognitive behavioral treatment such as the Coping Power program (Lochman and Wells 2002) targets these problem solving skills, for example by assisting children to better anticipate the consequences they may experience for their solutions to their social problems. It has been shown that this program for DBD in middle childhood is more powerful than usual care in reducing substance use in early adolescence (Zonnevylle-Bender et al. 2007). The present study suggests that risky decision making may be included in problem solving skills training in view of preventing SD in DBD children and adolescents.
In conclusion, the results of the present study suggest that increased risk taking instead of being a general characteristic of adolescents with DBD without ADHD co-morbidity, may be associated with the development of SD in a subgroup of these adolescents.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank all the volunteers and the following institutions: JJI De Heuvelrug, De Waag, Barentsz (Altrecht), De Derde Oever (De Bascule), De Fjord (ParnassioBavogroep). We would like to acknowledge Maria de Glint, Renske Houben, Azra Krdzalic, Kim van der Lelij and Marijke ter Veer for assistance in collecting the data. This work was supported by the ZonMw Addiction Program (3110001), the Netherlands, and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R01 DA 16135), United States of America. Disclosure: The authors report no conflicts of interest.
Open Access
This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Open AccessThis is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Noncommercial License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0), which permits any noncommercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author(s) and source are credited.
Met BSL Psychologie Totaal blijf je als professional steeds op de hoogte van de nieuwste ontwikkelingen binnen jouw vak. Met het online abonnement heb je toegang tot een groot aantal boeken, protocollen, vaktijdschriften en e-learnings op het gebied van psychologie en psychiatrie. Zo kun je op je gemak en wanneer het jou het beste uitkomt verdiepen in jouw vakgebied.