What is hyperfocus?
Why has hyperfocus been forgotten?
Clinical population | Author(s) | Description of hyperfocus |
---|---|---|
ADHD | Hupfeld et al. (2019)* | “While the estimated 8 million adults in the USA affected by attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) might find it nearly impossible to sit still in a lecture hall or excruciatingly challenging to focus on writing a term paper, these same individuals might find themselves spending hours at a time composing a new song, tinkering with their car, writing computer code, or watching television (Kessler et al., 2006). The term “hyperfocus” (HF) has been used to characterize this state of heightened, focused attention that individuals with ADHD frequently report (Brown, 2005; Conner, 1994; Ozel-Kizil et al., 2016).” |
Ozel-Kizil et al. (2016)* | “ ‘Hyperfocusing’ is defined as a clinical phenomenon of “locking on” to a task in patients with ADHD who have a difficulty of shifting their attention from one subject to another, especially if the subject is about their interests (Conner, 1994). Hyperfocusing was mentioned as a state resembling a “hypnotic spell”, according to the subjective experiences of the cases with ADHD (Brown, 2005)… Moreover, hyperfocused individuals neglect things other than the condition they are already focused on. Patients with ADHD are reported to be stuck in the activities that they are interested and they keep on doing these things for hours while they lose interest in their surroundings…The patients with ADHD usually report that they cannot understand how the time passes. During hyperfocusing, the individuals state that they are aware of the things that they ignore, however they cannot give up what they are doing (Brown, 2005; Conner, 1994). Hyperfocusing is thought to occur on the basis of attention disorder; patients with ADHD have difficulties of focusing and sustaining, as well as shifting their attention.” | |
Ozel-Kizil et al. (2013)* | “Hyperfocusing, which is characterized by intensive concentration on interesting and non-routine activities accompanied by temporarily diminished perception of the environment, is a clinically well-known phenomenon in patients with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. It is also described as ‘locking on’ to some task.” | |
Sklar (2013)* | “…hyperfocus appears to refer to a more specific (and perhaps extreme) type of sustained attention in which the individual’s behaviour is controlled for a long period of time by a task which is ‘non-routine’ or of interest to him/her, to the point that his/her awareness of the environment is considerably diminished.” | |
Goodwin and Oberacker (2011) | “Many children with ADHD have the ability to hyperfocus on certain tasks. This trait can confuse parents, as they see their highly distractible child engrossed in a video game, for example. They call his name but he has tuned them out, along with every other stimulus in the room.” | |
Schecklmann et al. (2008) | “Hyperfocusing is not mentioned in DSM-IV [with respect to ADHD], but it is known from clinical work and can be described as intensive concentration on interesting and non-routine activities accompanied by temporarily diminished perception of the environment.” | |
Carver (2009) | “Both research and clinical experience tells us that ADHD Children [sic] can exhibit a type of “hyperfocus”—intense concentration and single-minded focus when the activity is very interesting.” | |
Kahl and Wahl (2006) | “The researchers noted that “interest” probably the most frequently experience positive emotion, “is an extremely important motivation in the development of skills, competencies and intelligence”. The motivating power of such “interest” may be most apparent when it is absent, as described in the chronic complaints of many adults with ADDs who report that although they can “hyperfocus” on activities in which they have special interest, they chronically find themselves unable to mobilize effort for tasks in which they do not feel any special immediate interest, even when they are fully aware that their failure to do that uninteresting task may cause significant problems later.” | |
Autism | Isomura, Ogawa, Shibasaki, & Masataka, (2015) | “Typically, children with autism are known to… pay abnormal and obsessive attention to detail, and to note and record their environment with exquisite clarity (Casey et al., 2008). They are capable of becoming hyper-focused and locked-in on apparently arbitrary subjects of interest, and of sustaining their attention on these subjects for unusually long periods of time…as a result of this internal hyper-focus, it would be more difficult for another person to command the attention of the child with autism, and it would also be more difficult for the child himself/herself to command his/her own attention voluntarily (Posner and Dehaene, 1994).” |
Fein (2015) | “The co-existence of strength and vulnerability encapsulated in these narratives captured essential features of the experience of living with Asperger’s Syndrome—a condition that itself brought valued strengths (the ability to hyperfocus on a topic of interest, strengths in systematic thinking, an occasionally exquisite sensitivity to sensory input) as well as disabilities.” | |
Mayes (2014) | “Unlike most children with ADHD who have difficulty sustaining their focus on anything, children with autism can hyperfocus on activities of interest to them (e.g., spending hours twirling a string, assembling puzzles, drawing the same picture over and over, or reading a book).” | |
Meilleur, Jelenic, and Mottron, (2014) | “Alternatively, improvements in adaptive abilities may accompany loss of skills involving hyperfocus, as autistic people learn and adapt.” (No description of hyperfocus is provided and there is no further mention of it in the paper) | |
Bombaci (2012) | “Besides associational thinking and mindblindness, autistic subjects also tend to display extreme concentration when gazing on or thinking about objects that interest them. Other terms for autistic hyper-focus are ‘stimming’ and ‘perseveration’. Interestingly, over-selective attention, the clinical term for this form of perceptual difference, echoes William James’s notion of selective attention—a perceptual ability that he associated with masculine power.” | |
Mayes, Calhoun, Murray, Ahuja, and Smith, (2011) | “Attention deficit, hyperactivity, and impulsivity are common in children with autism, but, unlike children with ADHD, children with autism have the ability to hyperfocus on activities of interest to them, such as spending hours twirling a string or reading a book (Mayes & Calhoun, 1999). Repetitive behaviors in autism (e.g., spinning wheels on a car or drawing the same pictures over and over) are often driven by pleasure…” | |
Geurts et al. (2009) | “Difficulties in shifting attention, disengaging attention from details (i.e., hyperfocus)” | |
Schizophrenia | Luck et al. (2019)* | “This new hypothesis states that schizophrenia involves an aberrant hyperfocusing of processing resources on a small number of representations. In other words, even when the task requires perceiving or remembering multiple objects or locations, PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] tend to focus intensely but narrowly.” |
Sawaki et al. (2017)* | “…the hyperfocusing hypothesis, which proposes that PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] tend to focus their processing resources more intensely but more narrowly than HCS [healthy controls] as a result of disrupted attractor dynamics that tend to create deeper basins of attraction and produce exaggerated winner-take-all processing (Luck et al., 2014)” | |
Luck et al. (2014)* | “… processing resources are focused more intensely but more narrowly in PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] than in healthy control subjects (HCS [healthy controls]). In other words, PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] focus unusually strongly on some sources of information to the exclusion of others. We call this the hyperfocusing hypothesis.” | |
Gray et al. (2014)* | “We speculate that impairments in the Divided Attention subtest, and in part also reduced WM [working memory] capacity, reflect an underlying abnormality in the dynamics of local cortical circuits in PSZ [patients with schizophrenia]. Briefly, we propose that an imbalance between excitatory and inhibitory function tends to cause exaggerated local inhibition and an increase in winner-take-all processing. This winner-take-all processing mode is suggested to cause a ‘hyperfocusing’ of resources onto a small number of locations or objects, whether they are currently visible (as in the Divided Attention subtest) or being held in memory (as in our WM [working memory] task). When applied to external representations, the tendency to hyperfocus may lead to deficits in dividing attention among multiple targets or spreading attention among multiple locations or a broad area in space. When hyperfocusing is applied to internal representations, this would lead to a reduction in the number of items, rules, or response alternatives that can be simultaneously active, which could compromise more complex cognitive operations.” | |
Leonard et al. (2012)* | “Recent work has instead found that schizophrenia is associated with a ‘failure’ to attend broadly (Elahipanah et al. 2011; Hahn et al. 2012), suggesting that impaired WM [working memory] capacity estimates in PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] may reflect a tendency to hyperfocus on a subset of the relevant information rather than an inability to filter irrelevant information.” | |
Hahn et al. (2016)* | “…it has been suggested that PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] have a narrowed “attentional spotlight” and difficulty maintaining a wide visual span We followed up on these findings with a visuospatial Allocation Task (SARAT), in which a central cue predicts the location of a peripheral target stimulus… One, 2, or all 4 possible target locations could be cued simultaneously, manipulating the degree to which attention had to be focused narrowly or distributed broadly. Both HCS [healthy controls] and PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] displayed step-wise faster RT with more precise cueing. However, this effect was substantially larger in PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] than in HCS [healthy controls]. Potential explanations for this finding are that (1) PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] “hyperfocused” the location to which a predictive cue directed their attention, resulting in disproportionate RT benefits in predictive cue trials, or (2) PSZ [patients with schizophrenia] had difficulty distributing attention broadly, resulting in greater RT costs when there was no advance information about the target location.” | |
Prentky (2001) | “Positive symptoms, as Gruzelier and Raine (1994) reported, are associated with higher left than right hemispheric activity, supporting the hypothesis that general overactivation of the left hemisphere or underactivation of the right hemisphere characterizes the C-type. Thus, there is a hypothetical optimal hemispheric imbalance that promotes a constructive, task-specific hyperfocus on detail and facilitates problem solving but does not seriously incapacitate or debilitate the individual.” |
Hyperfocus and possibly related phenomena
Hyperfocus and flow
perceived challenges, or opportunities for action, that stretch but do not overmatch existing skills; clear proximal goals and immediate feedback about the progress being made.Under these conditions, experience seamlessly unfolds from moment to moment and one enters a subjective state with the following characteristics:
intense and focused concentration on the present moment; merging of action and awareness; loss of reflective self-consciousness (i.e., loss of awareness of oneself as a social actor); a sense that one can control one’s actions; that is, a sense that one can in principle deal with the situation because one knows how to respond to whatever happens next; distortion of temporal experience (typically a sense that time has passed faster than normal); experience of the activity as intrinsically rewarding, such that often the end goal is just an excuse for the process.
Hyperfocus criteria | Corresponding flow criteria/experiences |
---|---|
Hyperfocus is characterized by an intense state of concentration/focus | Intense and focused concentration on the present moment |
When engaged in hyperfocus, unrelated external stimuli do not appear to be consciously perceived; sometimes reported as a diminished perception of the environment | Merging of action and awareness |
Loss of reflective self-consciousness (i.e., loss of awareness of oneself as a social actor) | |
Distortion of temporal experience (typically a sense that time has passed faster than normal) | |
To engage in hyperfocus, the task has to be fun or interesting | Experience of the activity as intrinsically rewarding, such that often the end goal is just an excuse for the process |
Perceived challenges, or opportunities for action, that stretch but do not overmatch existing skills | |
During a hyperfocus state, task performance improves | A sense that one can control one’s actions; that is, a sense that one can in principle deal with the situation because one knows how to respond to whatever happens next |
Criterion 1: to engage a hyperfocus, the task has to be fun or interesting
Criteria 2 and 3: intense state of concentration; external stimuli do not appear to be consciously perceived/diminished perception of the environment
Criterion 4: task performance improves
The neural correlates of flow
Hyperfocus and being “In the Zone”
An operational definition of hyperfocus
