Skip to main content

Welkom bij Erasmus MC & Bohn Stafleu van Loghum

Erasmus MC heeft ervoor gezorgd dat je Mijn BSL eenvoudig en snel kunt raadplegen. Je kunt je links eenvoudig registreren. Met deze gegevens kun je thuis, of waar ook ter wereld toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Registreer

Om ook buiten de locaties van Erasmus MC, thuis bijvoorbeeld, van Mijn BSL gebruik te kunnen maken, moet je jezelf eenmalig registreren. Dit kan alleen vanaf een computer op een van de locaties van Erasmus MC.

Eenmaal geregistreerd kun je thuis of waar ook ter wereld onbeperkt toegang krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Login

Als u al geregistreerd bent, hoeft u alleen maar in te loggen om onbeperkt toegang te krijgen tot Mijn BSL.

Top

16-04-2025 | Original Article

Does Negative Judgment of Emotion Influence Mood and Rumination? Testing the Impact of a Judgment Induction Procedure

Auteurs: Samantha Moshier, Monifer Vaca Solares, Kira Jamieson

Gepubliceerd in: Cognitive Therapy and Research

Log in om toegang te krijgen
share
DELEN

Deel dit onderdeel of sectie (kopieer de link)

  • Optie A:
    Klik op de rechtermuisknop op de link en selecteer de optie “linkadres kopiëren”
  • Optie B:
    Deel de link per e-mail

Abstract

Purpose

The tendency to negatively judge one’s thoughts and feelings has been linked with depressive symptoms, but the mechanisms of this association are not well understood. To study the causal influence of judgment of one’s inner experience on emotional experiencing, we developed a lab procedure designed to induce a temporary sense of judgment of one’s negative emotions. We tested its effects on emotion intensity and state rumination following sad mood induction, and examined whether depressive symptom severity moderated these effects.

Methods

Participants were assigned to the judgment induction condition (n = 44) or control condition (n = 45). Those in the judgment induction condition read and summarized a sham advertisement describing the harmful impact of negative emotions, while the control group read and summarized a neutral infographic. Participants then underwent a sad mood induction, after which we measured state affect and judgment of emotion. After a 5-min no-task delay period designed to allow for engagement in spontaneous rumination, we assessed state rumination and re-assessed state affect.

Results

Following the experimental manipulation and mood induction, the judgment induction group reported higher negative judgment of emotion than the control group (b = 8.43, p =.034). There was a significant time by condition interaction on sadness ratings (F (1.92, 163.32) = 4.68, p =.012, ηp2 =.05); although both groups experienced a similar increase in sadness from pre- to immediately post-mood induction, the judgment induction group had not recovered from the mood induction as fully as controls five minutes later. Finally, condition interacted with depression symptom severity to predict state rumination (b = 7.05, p =.019). Specifically, relative to controls, the judgment induction led to increased rumination among those with greater depression symptom severity.

Conclusions

Findings support the validity of a procedure for inducing a temporary sense of negative judgment of one’s negative emotions. Judging one’s negative feelings may slow recovery from sad mood states, and among those with increased depression symptoms, may increase engagement in rumination.
Bijlagen
Alleen toegankelijk voor geautoriseerde gebruikers
Literatuur
go back to reference Conway, M., Csank, P. A., Holm, S. L., & Blake, C. K. (2000). On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75(3), 404–425.CrossRefPubMed Conway, M., Csank, P. A., Holm, S. L., & Blake, C. K. (2000). On assessing individual differences in rumination on sadness. Journal of Personality Assessment, 75(3), 404–425.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Corman, M., Rubio, M. T., Cabrespine, A., Brindel, I., Bay, J. O., De La Tour, R. P., & Dambrun, M. (2022). Mindful non-judging and experiential avoidance before allogeneic hematopoietic stem cell transplantation (HSCT) are associated to post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology 5 months later. Mindfulness. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-021-01788-1CrossRef Corman, M., Rubio, M. T., Cabrespine, A., Brindel, I., Bay, J. O., De La Tour, R. P., & Dambrun, M. (2022). Mindful non-judging and experiential avoidance before allogeneic hematopoietic stem cell transplantation (HSCT) are associated to post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptomatology 5 months later. Mindfulness. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1007/​s12671-021-01788-1CrossRef
go back to reference Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. Guilford Press. Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (2011). Acceptance and commitment therapy: The process and practice of mindful change. Guilford Press.
go back to reference Kearns, N. P., Shawyer, F., Brooker, J. E., Graham, A. L., Enticott, J. C., Martin, P. R., & Meadows, G. N. (2016). Does rumination mediate the relationship between mindfulness and depressive relapse? Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 89(1), 33–49.CrossRef Kearns, N. P., Shawyer, F., Brooker, J. E., Graham, A. L., Enticott, J. C., Martin, P. R., & Meadows, G. N. (2016). Does rumination mediate the relationship between mindfulness and depressive relapse? Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 89(1), 33–49.CrossRef
go back to reference Kuyken, W., Byford, S., Taylor, R. S., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Evans, A., Mullan, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2008). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(6), 966–978. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013786CrossRefPubMed Kuyken, W., Byford, S., Taylor, R. S., Watkins, E., Holden, E., White, K., Barrett, B., Byng, R., Evans, A., Mullan, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2008). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy to prevent relapse in recurrent depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76(6), 966–978. https://​doi.​org/​10.​1037/​a0013786CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lenth, R. V., Bolker, B., Buerkner, P., Gine-Vazquez, I., Herve, M., Jung, M., Love, J., Miguez, F., Piaskowski, J., Riebl, H., Singmann, H. (2024). “Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means.” R package version 1.10.4. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans Lenth, R. V., Bolker, B., Buerkner, P., Gine-Vazquez, I., Herve, M., Jung, M., Love, J., Miguez, F., Piaskowski, J., Riebl, H., Singmann, H. (2024). “Emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means.” R package version 1.10.4. https://​CRAN.​R-project.​org/​package=​emmeans
go back to reference Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.CrossRefPubMed Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of negative emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(3), 335–343.CrossRefPubMed
go back to reference Lundqvist, E. Flykt, D. A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces—KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institute ISBN 91-630-7164-9. Lundqvist, E. Flykt, D. A., & Öhman, A. (1998). The Karolinska directed emotional faces—KDEF, CD ROM from Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Psychology section, Karolinska Institute ISBN 91-630-7164-9.
go back to reference Marchetti, I., Mor, N., Chiorri, C., & Koster, E. H. (2018). The brief state rumination inventory (BSRI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(4), 447–460.CrossRef Marchetti, I., Mor, N., Chiorri, C., & Koster, E. H. (2018). The brief state rumination inventory (BSRI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(4), 447–460.CrossRef
go back to reference Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (2006). Extending the goal progress theory of rumination: Goal re-evaluation and growth. In L. J. Sanna & E. C. Chang (Eds.), Judgments over time: The interplay of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (pp. 145–162). Oxford University Press.CrossRef Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (2006). Extending the goal progress theory of rumination: Goal re-evaluation and growth. In L. J. Sanna & E. C. Chang (Eds.), Judgments over time: The interplay of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (pp. 145–162). Oxford University Press.CrossRef
go back to reference Segal, Z., Williams, M., & Teasdale, J. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression. Guilford Press. Segal, Z., Williams, M., & Teasdale, J. (2012). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression. Guilford Press.
Metagegevens
Titel
Does Negative Judgment of Emotion Influence Mood and Rumination? Testing the Impact of a Judgment Induction Procedure
Auteurs
Samantha Moshier
Monifer Vaca Solares
Kira Jamieson
Publicatiedatum
16-04-2025
Uitgeverij
Springer US
Gepubliceerd in
Cognitive Therapy and Research
Print ISSN: 0147-5916
Elektronisch ISSN: 1573-2819
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-025-10603-3